Introduction

Memory can be defined as an experience-dependent alteration in behavior that persists beyond the environmental stimuli that produced it. Memory is often conceptualized as a multi-staged process that includes encoding, consolidation, retrieval, and forgetting. As such, mechanistically interpreting memory in the brain is facilitated by understanding the neural underpinnings of each of these stages independently, as well as how these neural elements interrelate across stages. In this regard, significant progress has been made in our understanding memory stages at the level of ‘engram neurons’ – that is, neurons that mediate a particular memory across stages [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8].

In this review, we seek to identify and connect key overarching principles — principles that seem to largely hold across neural regions and tasks — that lead to neurons participating across multiple memory stages and forming a cellular substrate of memory. Our review focuses primarily on rodent research from the hippocampus, amygdala, and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) [9], and is organized according to a typical order of memory stages: encoding, consolidation, retrieval, and forgetting (Fig. 1). We note that many excellent reviews on memory have been written on these memory stages (e.g. [1,2,3, 5, 8, 10,11,12]; and note other memory stages exist outside of this scope: e.g. [13,14,15]). Our goal here is to complement and build upon previous work by synthesizing understanding of engram neurons, both within and across these stages of memory. Motivated by recent empirical developments, we conclude our review by suggesting some important conceptual extensions as to how the engram is traditionally examined and understood.

Fig. 1: Schematic of cellular and synaptic organization across memory stages.
figure 1

The transformation of the engram is depicted for each stage of memory summarized in this review.

Memory encoding

Intrinsic neuronal excitability regulates recruitment into the engram

The engram can be viewed as the physical change that occurred in the nervous system in response to a learned experience, which can later mediate instantiation of the corresponding memory. As such, engram neurons are typically defined as the neurons that are preferentially involved in the encoding, consolidation, and retrieval of a particular memory [8] (Box 1).

Why are some neurons, rather than their neighbors, recruited during the encoding of a memory? In principle, neurons might be preferentially recruited in memory encoding due to specialized intrinsic properties. Reinforcing this, intrinsic cellular excitability — the propensity of a neuron to fire an action potential in response to input — can be a key determinant of participation in memory [16]. In the context of memory, neurons with high excitability can be biased towards responding during learning and participating in memory encoding [16,17,18], illustrating in certain cases that intrinsic excitability can bias cells to being active during memory encoding.

To examine this principle from an interventional perspective, the transcription factor cAMP Response Element-Binding protein (CREB) is often leveraged as a tool to regulate neuronal activity during memory encoding [12, 19,20,21]. CREB regulates neuronal activity in a variety of subcortical and cortical regions [22,23,24,25,26], and across an array of memory tasks [26,27,28,29,30], and can thus regulate which neurons are allocated into the engram. Specifically, CREB-enhanced neurons are preferentially recruited into the engram, whereas CREB-deficient neurons are actively inhibited from encoding the memory [26, 31, 32]. Importantly, exogenous enhancement of CREB minutes before learning is sufficient to promote memory [33], thereby illustrating that CREB activity can shape memory on brief and behaviourally relevant timescales.

Such CREB-dependent recruitment is often posited to reflect changes in intrinsic neural excitability, with these changes in excitability sha** allocation during memory encoding. This posit has been empirically demonstrated in some cases, wherein selective suppression of the excitability of CREB-enhanced neurons prevents their preferential recruitment into the engram, whereas increasing excitability in a subset of neurons without manipulating levels of CREB enhances their recruitment into the engram [34]. This excitability-based mechanism of memory allocation is recapitulated under physiological conditions, with natural fluctuations in rates of neural excitability determining which neurons are selected to encode the corresponding memory [18]. It should be noted that the timescales of excitability plasticity changes can be faster than that of that CREB activity changes, and thus the extent to which natural fluctuations in CREB dictate allocation into the engram has yet to be demonstrated. Finally, it should also be noted that CREB shapes a host of disparate cellular functions, including synaptic plasticity [35]. Thus, CREB-driven neural activity may reflect changes beyond intrinsic properties, and its effects on intrinsic neuronal excitability may only account for some aspects of selection of neurons for memory encoding (Box 2). Ultimately, the extent to which endogenous CREB acts directly on excitability, and the CREB-dependent downstream cellular mechanisms that govern excitability, will be important avenues in future research.

The idea that intrinsic neuronal excitability plays an important role in memory allocation receives additional support from work in place cells [36, 37]. Activating place cells at a specific location promotes the formation of a place field corresponding to that location [38, 39] and targeted activation of place cells drives memory-guided spatial behavior [40], indicating that place cells form an essential neuronal underpinning of spatial memory. Most relevant to the current discussion, only a small subset of neurons become place cells during spatial learning, with most remaining silent [36, 41]. What dictates whether a neuron will become a place cell versus a non-responding silent cell? CA1 neurons that become place cells display higher rates of excitability from the beginning of exploration, and sometimes even before the animal is introduced to the new environment [42]. Remarkably, silent cells can be transformed into place cells with spatially tuned place fields by lowering their activation thresholds [43], potentially suggesting that increased intrinsic excitability may result in place cell emergence under physiological conditions. These data provide complementary CREB-independent support for the idea that the relative excitability of neurons at the time of learning helps determine which neurons will encode the corresponding memory, indicating that this may be a general principle of memory allocation.

In collection, while these findings converge on neural excitability regulating cellular recruitment during memory encoding, they do not negate that other factors influence memory encoding as well. Current evidence illustrates pre-existing patterns of synaptic connectivity (Fig. 1) and synaptic consolidation alongside neuromodulatory factors (Box 2) likely play essential roles here as well. Such mechanisms may emerge from, as well as be complemented by, epigenetic and other cell-intrinsic molecular properties that are engaged during learning and bias neurons towards an enduring role in memory [44,45,46,47]. Thus, while work on CREB and neuronal excitability has played (and will continue to play) a foundational role in our understanding of memory allocation, an important avenue for future research is uncovering complementary factors that predispose neurons to be an element of the engram.

Intrinsic neural excitability mediates formation of neuronal ensembles

Thus far, we have discussed the selection process determining which individual neurons are allocated into a memory. However, memory is often thought to be represented not at the level of individual neurons, but at the level of neuronal ensembles – that is, neuronal populations that show consistently synchronized activity in response to a particular stimulus, function, or mental state (e.g., a memory) [48]. Do neuronal ensembles effectively embody independent neurons, or are there specific interrelationships between neurons comprising an ensemble? Two-photon holographic optogenetics has been used to address this question in a highly specific manner [49,50,51]. Repetitive two-photon optogenetic activation of groups of neurons increases the probability of their firing together in the absence of external stimulus, consistent with the formation of a neuronal ensemble [50]. Fascinatingly, these neuronal ensembles are formed via cell-intrinsic upregulation of neural excitability between stimulated neurons, without a concomitant increase in synaptic plasticity (i.e., no new synaptic connections were made between previously unconnected neurons) [51]. These results suggest that memory formation occurs when highly excitable neurons display coordinated activity during memory encoding [4], and that the corresponding neuronal ensemble is formed based on levels of intrinsic excitability rather than alterations in synaptic plasticity per se (Box 2). While future work is needed to clarify these results (e.g., determining the extent to which they generalize across learning conditions), one interpretation is that highly excitable neurons increase the efficiency of already-existing synapses (Box 2). While these findings highlight the role of neuronal excitability in the selection and formation of neuronal ensembles, they do not negate the role of synaptic plasticity in the stabilization or consolidation of these ensembles once they are created. Indeed, in the time following the formation of a neuronal ensemble that underlies a memory, mechanisms of synaptic plasticity would be engaged to consolidate and strengthen this ensemble for future use.

Encoding summary

A competition-based rule can account for initial memory allocation, wherein excitable neurons and their associated neuronal ensembles out-compete less excitable counterparts for recruitment into the engram. Converging evidence for this rule has been obtained across an array of memory assays and neural regions. This rule therefore seems to be a generalizable feature of learning, and thus key for understanding the mechanisms underlying memory encoding.

Memory consolidation

Memory persistence requires synaptic consolidation

Recently encoded memories can be temporarily maintained via learning-induced increased activity [16, 51,52,53,54,55]. However, these memories are labile, highly susceptible to interference, and will rapidly decay without additional maintenance. The transformation of a short-term, labile memory into one that persists long-term requires gene expression and de novo protein synthesis. These processes culminate in increased synaptic coupling between active neurons co-active at the time of learning – a phenomenon called synaptic consolidation [56, 57]. Relatedly, manipulations that disrupt the molecular cascades involved in synaptic consolidation prevent memory consolidation [58, 59]. For example, through its influence on synaptic plasticity, suppressing CREB activity inhibits memory consolidation [57, 60]. Likewise, protein synthesis inhibitors prevent consolidation of memories if administered soon after learning [56, 57]. As such, synaptic consolidation represents a critical point of divergence, wherein consolidated memories survive and have the potential to be retrieved in the future, while those that aren’t targeted for synaptic consolidation may be lost (but see [61]).

Synaptic consolidation occurs preferentially in neurons active during learning

Neurons preferentially engaged during learning (i.e., putative engram neurons) selectively exhibit hallmark features of synaptic consolidation following memory encoding. For example, transcriptomic analysis has revealed a highly enriched CREB-dependent network that is recruited in engram neurons following contextual fear conditioning [62]. This CREB-dependent transcription promotes structural and functional changes preferentially in engram neurons, and is required for synaptic consolidation [62] (see Box 2). For example, GluR1 AMPA-Rs are preferentially expressed in dendritic spines of active CA1 neurons following contextual fear conditioning [63], and dentate gyrus engram neurons display increased spine density and synaptic strength following contextual fear conditioning [61]. Protein synthesis inhibitors administered immediately after learning abolish these engram-selective changes and culminate in failed memory consolidation [61]. Additionally, synaptic potentiation and the number and size of dendritic spines is selectively increased in engram-to-engram CA3-to-CA1 synapses following formation of a contextual fear memory [64]. Taken together, these results suggest that synaptic consolidation at the molecular, structural, and functional level occur selectively in the neurons active in response to a learning experience.

Neuronal reactivation drives early consolidation

The probability of forming a long-term hippocampal-dependent memory increases upon repeated behavioural exposures to the learning event, and intriguingly, repeated internal representations of the learning event also occur during behaviourally ‘offline’ periods. During these offline times, such as sleep or quiet wakefulness, patterns of activity among recently active hippocampal neurons is spontaneously replayed. Such replay events occur in either a forward or backward direction [65, 66] in a temporally-compressed format – upwards of 20x faster than occurred during the initial learning experience [67, 68]. Hippocampal replay events occur selectively during sharp wave ripples (a form of high frequency network oscillation) and drive memory consolidation [67, 69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76]. For example, optogenetic increase of sharp wave ripple duration improves consolidation of the corresponding memory [73], and selective disruption of replay prevents consolidation of the corresponding memory [77]. Furthermore, memory replay doesn’t simply reflect the strongest representation rising to the surface, but often occurs for memories most in need of consolidation (i.e., those most at risk degradation) [78, 79]. In accordance with this, targeted reactivation of fear-conditioning-induced lateral amygdala engram neurons during consolidation increases subsequent memory strength [80] (for conceptually similar results in the retrosplenial cortex, see [81]). Moreover, these fear memory engram neurons are preferentially reactivated during sleep, and optogenetically inhibiting their reactivation during sleep (but not later waking periods) prevents memory consolidation [82,83,84]. These findings converge on the idea that internally generated replay strengthens recently formed memories.

Hippocampal-dependent memories undergo systems-level consolidation

In the days, weeks, and months (and potentially years, in humans) following synaptic consolidation, the initially hippocampal-dependent component of memory undergoes extreme reorganization and redistribution such that it can be stored and expressed in a hippocampal-independent, mPFC-dependent format. This spatial reorganization of memory is known as systems consolidation. While hippocampal and mPFC-neocortical ensembles representing the same experience can co-exist in the brain [85,86,87], the phenomenological (or subjective) qualities of the memory depend on which neuronal ensemble is activated. Hippocampal-dependent memories are context-specific and detailed (i.e., episodic), whereas mPFC-dependent memories are associated with a more gist-like quality [87,88,89,90,91,86]. That memory retrieval can be induced by activating engram neurons has been widely replicated across neural regions [3, 5, 8] and behavioural paradigms (e.g., fear conditioning [61, 121]; conditioned place avoidance [123] and preference [118], go/no-go licking [49], inhibitory avoidance [114], object location memory [114], social preference memory [124]).

Memory retrieval can promote a transient increase in engram excitability that causally drives improved memory performance [125]. In this work involving contextual discrimination, memory retrieval promotes excitability for hours in dentate gyrus engram neurons. During the period of elevated engram excitability, animals display improved memory flexibility and accuracy in terms of pattern separation and completion at the behavioral level. Mechanistically, this retrieval-induced increase in engram excitability and the corresponding improvement in memory performance is cell-intrinsic, driven by changes in the inwardly rectifying potassium channel Kir2.1. Through its comprehensive measurements and direct manipulations of neuronal excitability, this study helps set the standard for casual relationships between engram excitability and memory processing [125].

To uncover robust effects related to memory, most studies in the field seek to manipulate large numbers of engram neurons. In this context, it is striking that optogenetic stimulation of as few as two visual cortex engram neurons is sufficient in driving pattern completion of the neuronal ensemble to which the neurons belong and retrieval of the corresponding memory [49]. Similarly, activation of hippocampal engram neurons promotes the reactivation of (non-stimulated) engram neurons in the amygdala and throughout the cortex [100, 126] (for an indexing theory interpretation of these results, see Box 3). Thus, while the stimulation protocols used in engram activation experiments are focal and largely non-physiological, their effects on the brain are widespread and recapitulate natural patterns of neuronal activity in areas downstream of the stimulated region of interest. The brain’s ability to complete patterns of activity (e.g., brain-wide engram) from incomplete input (e.g., dentate gyrus engram stimulation) likely explains the ability of focal stimulation of engram neurons to drive memory retrieval (Box 4).

Retrieval summary

According to the encoding specificity principle, memory retrieval success is dictated by the extent to which cues present at retrieval match that those present during encoding. Consistent with this idea, observational experiments illustrate memory retrieval is associated with reactivation of engram neurons engaged during initial learning. Interventionally, silencing neurons that were active during encoding suppresses memory retrieval, whereas activating these neurons promotes memory retrieval. These results hold across an array of tasks and neural regions, suggesting that the engram that is formed during encoding and strengthened during consolidation can be correlatively and causally linked to memory retrieval.

Forgetting

Forgetting as an adaptive phenomenon

Forgetting is often viewed as the lack of behavioural expression of a memory, which could be otherwise successfully recalled and expressed on an earlier occasion [11, 127]. According to this perspective, forgetting can occur because the memory is no longer available (i.e., complete engram degradation; a storage deficit) or because it is not currently accessible (i.e., a retrieval deficit). We note that our operational definition of forgetting here, which can be explained by both storage and retrieval failure, is assayed by behaviour and is agnostic to its biological cause. This varies from some stricter theoretical treatments wherein forgetting requires loss of memory representation per se.

Why do we forget? Memories are perhaps best understood as models of the future [94, 128], and once a memory no longer services predicting what the future might be like, it is best forgotten [129]. While forgetting can occur in a passive manner, for example in response to interfering environmental stimuli, it can also be a well-regulated and active process [130]. Indeed, it has been argued that the natural tendency of neural systems is to degrade rather than preserve information [130, 131]. While forgetting has negative connotations, it is an adaptive phenomenon that promotes future mnemonic processing, decision making, emotional regulation, and mental health [94, 132, 133].

Synaptic remodeling: A general principle of forgetting

Synaptic plasticity is required for successful learning, but comes at the cost of potentially degrading information already stored in the circuit (i.e. the plasticity-stability dilemma [134]). Given sufficient synaptic remodeling of an engram-specific neuronal ensemble, forgetting is unavoidable: there is an inevitable tip** point beyond which information stored in the ensemble will be lost (i.e., unless another set of synapses takes over the memory representation [135, 136]). This conclusion follows naturally from the encoding specificity principle, and in particular state-dependent memory: as connectivity changes accumulate in an ensemble, the probability of faithfully recapitulating the pattern of activity that underlie memory retrieval within that ensemble diminishes. In this way, synaptic remodeling of engram circuitry represents a general principle of how forgetting occurs in the brain (for review, see [137]).

Neurogenesis-mediated synaptic remodeling and forgetting

Any significant alteration to the synaptic connectivity within which an engram is embedded should lead to forgetting of the corresponding memory. Post-learning hippocampal neurogenesis is a powerful means through which hippocampal circuitry is remodeled and altered: as new dentate gyrus granule neurons mature, they infiltrate and reconfigure surrounding circuitry by forming connections with both presynaptic and post-synaptic partners [11]. As newborn neurons integrate into these pre-established circuits, their synaptic connections exist alongside and, in some cases, replace established synaptic connections [138,139,140]. In kee** with their capacity to remodel surrounding neural circuitry, post-learning hippocampal neurogenesis reduces engram reinstatement in downstream CA3 and CA1 [141] and promotes forgetting of the corresponding hippocampal-dependent memory [142,143,170,171,172] and highlights Rac1 as a key player in this process.

Neurophysiological noise-induced forgetting: engram reactivation without remembering

Neurophysiological noise that co-occurs during a memory retrieval attempt decreases signal-to-noise ratio and promotes forgetting [173]. For example, altering patterns of synaptic weights via LTP induction in hippocampal synapses promotes forgetting of hippocampal-dependent spatial memories [173]. Moreover, optogenetic or chemogenetic activation of non-engram neurons during memory retrieval promotes forgetting of memories dependent on that circuitry [49, 174,175,176]. In a somewhat counter-intuitive finding, neurophysiological noise at retrieval often promotes forgetting without decreasing rates of engram neuron reactivation [174, 175](but see [49]). Thus, whereas synaptic remodeling promotes forgetting by decreasing the probability of engram reactivation, neurophysiological noise can interfere with memory retrieval without preventing engram activation. The precise nature of this interference is unknown but likely involves a reduction in memory-related information flow between neural regions. More generally, these results suggest that potentiating synapses that are independent of the engram decreases signal-to-noise ratio, interferes with memory retrieval, and culminates in forgetting.

Role of engram availability and accessibility in forgetting

The results outlined above suggest that forgetting can occur because the memory is no longer available (i.e., engram degradation; a storage deficit) or because it is not currently accessible (i.e., a retrieval failure) [177]. Memories are often retrievable in situations where one might classically assume that the engram has degraded to the point where it is no longer available [61]. For example, memories ‘lost’ to infantile amnesia [126, 178] (for related work on infantile amnesia, see [179,180,181]) and neurogenesis-induced forgetting [141] can be recovered by optogenetic or chemogenetic stimulation of the dentate gyrus engram. Likewise, forgetting in transgenic mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease can be reversed via dentate gyrus engram stimulation [114, 115]. Work that has examined both memory recovery and spine dynamics has found that synaptic strength and spine density can be reduced to baseline levels in hippocampal engram neurons, but nonetheless memory can be recovered via dentate gyrus engram stimulation [61]. Relatedly, selective optogenetic-induced depression of engram synapses induces forgetting, whereas potentiation of these synapses reinstates the memory [170,171,172, 182].

These results indicate that forgetting is often the result of failed memory retrieval, as opposed to memory erasure. What is the neurobiological explanation for the survival of memory after such drastic synaptic rearrangements and loss of synaptic strength? There are at least three ways of explaining these data. One, some memory-associated synapses remain, and these spared synapses (whether within the targeted neural circuit or in downstream neural regions) are sufficient in storing the memory but not in driving memory retrieval behavior under physiological conditions. Two, the loss of engram-specific synaptic strength diminishes access to information stored in the circuit, but the information stored in the circuit can survive this loss of synaptic strength via persistence in engram-specific synaptic connectivity. According to this explanation, there is a critical distinction between the synaptic strength required for memory retrieval, and the synaptic connectivity required for memory storage (for further discussion and elaboration, see [3, 6, 61, 183, 184]). Three, while highly speculative, it remains possible that non-synaptic mechanisms may be capable of long-term memory storage [3, 47, 185, 186] (Box 5).

Forgetting summary

As synaptic changes in engram circuitry accumulate, so too does the probability of forgetting. In this way, synaptic remodeling of engram circuitry represents a general mechanism of forgetting. Such synaptic remodeling can occur from a variety of sources, including depotentiation of existing synapses, new synapses driven by ongoing neurogenesis, and synaptic elimination by non-neuronal cells. By disrupting the properties of engram synapses strengthened during early memory stages, circuit remodeling decreases the probability of engram reactivation and promotes forgetting. Nonetheless, engram stimulation experiments can evoke memory retrieval under certain conditions, illustrating that such remodeling does not necessarily produce complete memory erasure per se.

Future Considerations

Conceptual underpinnings and extensions of the engram

Engram cells are often conceived as neurons that (a) are active during initial learning, (b) display some persistent physical change in response to learning, and (c) are reactivated during (and required for) memory retrieval [1, 2, 8]. According to this strict definition, no new neurons are added or removed from the engram after it is formed, since these neurons did not participate in both encoding and retrieval (although we note that some definitions incorporate dynamicism; e.g. [7]). Viewed from this perspective, the engram is perceived as a relatively rigid and unchanging neurobiological entity – a fact that seems at odds with the inherently dynamic and constructive nature of memory. Considering this fact, we highlight a few key generalizations regarding the nature of engram cells, motivated by recent experimental progress: (1) non-neuronal engram cells exist, (2) actively inhibited neurons can be an essential component of the engram, (3) different engram neurons can contribute to different stages in memory processing, (4) determining the essential differences (as opposed to only the commonalities) between encoding and retrieval engrams is important to advance the field (i.e., encoding ensemble reactivation is an incomplete model of memory retrieval).

  1. (1)

    Existence of non-neuronal engram cells. Generally, the engram field places a heavy, almost exclusive emphasis on neuronal engram cells. However, it is highly likely that non-neuronal engrams exist, with astrocytes being a prime candidate. For example, emerging evidence suggests that astrocytes play an active role in information process, including regulating synaptic function, circuit connectivity, and memory retrieval [150, 187]. In addition, activation of astrocytes during memory encoding improves memory retrieval without altering basal synaptic transmission [188]. Similarly, the location of a mouse in a familiar maze can be predicted from astrocyte activity alone, suggesting that these cell types might directly encode spatial information [189]. The extent to which astrocytes are instructive engram cells (in addition to being permissive supporting cells) is worthy of serious consideration and experimentation.

  2. (2)

    Neural inactivity does not imply mnemonic passivity. A complete neurobiological understanding of the engram will include not just active neurons, but also actively inhibited neurons. Such active inhibition is often necessary for memory. As one example, the anterodorsal thalamic nucleus is necessary for the retrieval of recent, but not remote, contextual fear memory [190]. Notably, the anterodorsal thalamic nucleus needs to be actively inhibited at remote timepoints for memory retrieval to succeed [190]. Because this inhibition is necessary for content-specific memory retrieval success, these inhibited neurons ought to be considered a genuine component of the engram. Similarly, much as re-activating neurons that were active during encoding promotes memory retrieval, re-inhibiting neurons that were actively inhibited during encoding can also promote memory retrieval [191]. That such neurons are re-inhibited (rather than re-activated) during memory retrieval does not preclude them from being an essential component of the engram (for conceptually related work on inhibitory engrams, see [192, 193]).

  3. (3)

    Different neurons often underlie different stages of memory processing. Engram neurons are typically defined as neurons that were active during both encoding and retrieval of memory. Emerging evidence has illustrated, however, that some neurons play a critical role in memory encoding but do not have a similarly critical role in memory retrieval [194,195,196]. Conversely, there are neurons that play no clear role in memory encoding, but are recruited into the engram later during consolidation and contribute significantly to memory retrieval [98, 119, 194]. As such, a less strict definition of the engram may serve to help to amalgamate these complementary roles, incorporating ‘encoding engram neurons’ (i.e., neurons that are essential for memory encoding only), ‘retrieval engram neurons’ (i.e., neurons that are essential for memory retrieval only), and ‘reactivated engram neurons’ (i.e., neurons essential to both encoding and retrieval). Such terminology more accurately captures the dynamic nature of memory [7, 197, 198], and better highlights the role different engram neurons play in different stages of memory processing. Conceptually reframing engram neurons in this way could result in new and important research questions. As examples, what are the mechanisms and environmental factors that mediate the recruitment of new neurons into a pre-existing engram? What information is carried by neurons that participate in either encoding or retrieval, but not both?

  4. (4)

    ‘Encoding’ engram reactivation is an incomplete model of memory retrieval. Memory is an inherently constructive process. In kee** with this, perception of an experience and memory retrieval of that experience are fundamentally distinct phenomena, with distinct psychological properties, and which must therefore engage – at least in part – distinct neural circuitry [199]. The engram field (including the current article) focuses almost exclusively on the commonalities between engram activation at encoding vs retrieval (i.e., engram reactivation) – and this remains a topic worthy of intensive study. However, it is equally important to study and understand the neurobiological differences between neural activity during memory encoding vs retrieval. Rather than being interpreted exclusively as noise or mnemonic imprecision, these differences in engram (in)activity could represent important (and adaptive) differences in how the brain processes perceptual information during encoding vs mnemonic information during retrieval.

Concluding remarks

The engram field, driven by new technology in combination with clever experimental design, has had a truly remarkable rate of recent discovery. The field is now able to visualize, measure, and manipulate engram neurons with an impressive level of specificity, enabling the role and evolution of the engram to be understood across memory stages. Such research has paved the way for exciting future opportunities to understand the engram across memory stages, in both traditional and non-canonical ways, and reveal the logic of memory in the brain.