Log in

The Tripartite Model of Risk Perception (TRIRISK): Distinguishing Deliberative, Affective, and Experiential Components of Perceived Risk

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Annals of Behavioral Medicine

Abstract

Background

Although risk perception is a key predictor in health behavior theories, current conceptions of risk comprise only one (deliberative) or two (deliberative vs. affective/experiential) dimensions.

Purpose

This research tested a tripartite model that distinguishes among deliberative, affective, and experiential components of risk perception.

Method

In two studies, and in relation to three common diseases (cancer, heart disease, diabetes), we used confirmatory factor analyses to examine the factor structure of the tripartite risk perception (TRIRISK) model and compared the fit of the TRIRISK model to dual-factor and single-factor models. In a third study, we assessed concurrent validity by examining the impact of cancer diagnosis on (a) levels of deliberative, affective, and experiential risk perception, and (b) the strength of relations among risk components, and tested predictive validity by assessing relations with behavioral intentions to prevent cancer.

Results

The tripartite factor structure was supported, producing better model fit across diseases (studies 1 and 2). Inter-correlations among the components were significantly smaller among participants who had been diagnosed with cancer, suggesting that affected populations make finer-grained distinctions among risk perceptions (study 3). Moreover, all three risk perception components predicted unique variance in intentions to engage in preventive behavior (study 3).

Conclusions

The TRIRISK model offers both a novel conceptualization of health-related risk perceptions, and new measures that enhance predictive validity beyond that engendered by unidimensional and bidimensional models. The present findings have implications for the ways in which risk perceptions are targeted in health behavior change interventions, health communications, and decision aids.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price includes VAT (Germany)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The reliabilities and ranges for the TRIRISK scales were similar for the cancer vs. no cancer groups. Among participants with no diagnosis of cancer, the standardized (z-score) deliberative scale (α = 0.917) has a range of −2.26–2.38, and skewness of −0.054 (SE = 0.193);, the standardized (z-score) affective scale (α = 0.972) has a range of −1.88–1.51, and skewness of −0.080 (SE = 0.193); and, the standardized (z-score) experiential scale (α = 0.873) has a range of −2.77–1.62, and skewness of −0.353 (SE = 0.193). Among those with cancer, the standardized (z-score) deliberative scale (α = 0.832) has a range of −2.23–2.29, and skewness of −0.248 (SE = 0.203); the standardized (z-score) affective scale (α = 0.967) has a range of −1.88–1.51, and skewness of −0.469 (SE = 0.203); and the standardized (z-score) experiential scale (α = 0.841) has a range of −2.77–1.62, and skewness of −0.716 (SE = 0.203). To test the scale distributions for the cancer vs. no cancer groups, we divided skewness values by their standard errors. Only two values exceeded the threshold of 1.96, namely, the affective and experiential scales for participants who had been diagnosed with cancer. We therefore transformed these variables which reduced skewness to −0.266 and −0.216, SE = 0.203 and 0.203, respectively [52]. We then recomputed the correlations; r values were virtually unchanged.

  2. We also tested whether the three risk components statistically mediated the impact of cancer diagnosis (yes, no) on intention using the Preacher and Hayes SPSS Macro for Multiple Mediation with 5000 as the number of bootstrap resamples [53]. Diagnosis reliably predicted deliberative, affective, and experiential risk perceptions (B = 0.44, 0.42, and 0.51, SE = 0.11, 0.11. and 0.11, ps < 0.001) and deliberative, affective, and experiential risk perceptions reliably predicted intention (B = −0.76, 0.48, and 0.34, SE = 0.09, 0.10, and 0.11, ps < 0.001, < 0.001, and 0.003). The direct effect of diagnosis on intention remained significant controlling for risk perceptions (B = 0.36, SE = 0.14, p = 0.013). Importantly, however, the indirect effects of deliberative, affective, and experiential risk perceptions each proved reliable (estimates = −0.34, 0.20, and 0.17; 95 % CI = −0.55 to −0.17, 0.09 to 0.36, and 0.06 to 0.35, respectively). These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that the three components of the TRIRISK model mediate the impact of cancer diagnosis on cancer-protective intentions.

  3. Because (a) some of the deliberative risk perceptions could be seen as conditional on current behavior (e.g., “The way I look after my health means that my odds of getting cancer in the future are . . .”), and (b) conditional risk perceptions can be a strong predictor of future behavior, we examined correlations between each individual deliberative risk perception item and intentions in the full sample to see if items performed similarly. Items 3, 4, and 5 each had a statistically significant negative correlation with intentions (rs = −0.254, −0.183, −0.264, respectively; all ps < 0.001). However, item 2 also had a marginally significant, negative association with intentions (r = −0.108, p = 0.062); the other items had non-significant correlations with intentions that were mostly negative. Thus, while conditional items have some role in explaining the negative correlation between deliberative risk perception and intention, this does not entirely explain the negative correlation between deliberative risk and intention.

  4. We also tested the interactions between cancer diagnosis and risk components but none of the interaction terms proved reliable. Deliberative, affective, and experiential risk perceptions were equally powerful and reliable predictors of intention for participants with and without a diagnosis of cancer.

References

  1. Becker MH. The health belief model and sick role behavior. Health EducMonogr. 1974; 2: 409-419.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Rogers RW. A protection motivation theory of fear appeals and attitude change. J Psychol: Interdiscy Appl. 1975; 91: 93-114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Rosenstock IM. Historical origins of the health belief model. Health Educ Behav. 1974; 2: 328-335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Slovic P. Assessment of risk taking behavior. Psychol Bull. 1964; 61: 220.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Weinstein ND. The precaution adoption process. Health Psychol. 1988; 7: 355.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Shafir E, Simonson I, Tversky A. Reason-based choice. Cogn. 1993; 49: 11-36.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Tversky A, Kahneman D. Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychol Rev. 1983; 90: 293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Damasio A: Descartes’ error: Emotion, rationality, and the human brain. New York, NY: Putnam, 1994/2003.

  9. Kunda Z. The case for motivated reasoning. Psychol Bull. 1990; 108: 480-498.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Denes-Raj V, Epstein S. Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: When people behave against their better judgment. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994; 66: 819-829.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. von Neumann J, Morgenstern O. Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Loewenstein G, Weber EU, Hsee CK, Welch N. Risk as feelings. Psychol Bull. 2001; 127: 267-286.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Brewer NT, Chapman GB, Gibbons FX, et al. Meta-analysis of the relationship between risk perception and health behavior: The example of vaccination. Health Psychol. 2007; 26: 136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Brewer NT, Weinstein ND, Cuite CL, Herrington JE Jr. Risk perceptions and their relation to risk behavior. Ann Behav Med. 2004; 27: 125-130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Sheeran P, Harris PR, Epton T. Does heightening risk appraisals change people’s intentions and behavior?A meta-analysis of experimental studies. Psychol Bull. 2014; 140: 511.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, MacGregor DG. Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Anal. 2004; 24: 311-322.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Leventhal H, Meyer D, Merenz D. The common sense representation of illness danger. Contrib Med Psychol. 1980; 2: 7-30.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Leventhal H. Findings and theory in the study of fear communications. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 1970; 5: 119-186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Dillard AJ, Ferrer RA, Ubel PA, Fagerlin A. Risk perception measures’ associations with behavior intentions, affect, and cognition following colon cancer screening messages. Health Psychol. 2012; 31: 106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Ferrer RA, Hall KL, Portnoy DB, et al. Relationships among health perceptions vary depending on stage of readiness for colorectal cancer screening. Health Psychol. 2011; 30: 525-535.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Lipkus IM, Klein WMP, Skinner CS, Rimer BK. Breast cancer risk perceptions and breast cancer worry: What predicts what? J Risk Res. 2005; 8: 439-452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Hay JL, McCaul KD, Magnan RE. Does worry about breast cancer predict screening behaviors? A meta-analysis of the prospective evidence. Prev Med. 2006; 42: 401-408.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Ferrer RA, Portnoy DB, Klein WM. Worry and risk perceptions as independent and interacting predictors of health protective behaviors. J Health Commun. 2013; 18: 397-409.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Sinclair M, Ashkanasy NM, Chattopadhyay P. Affective antecedents of intuitive decision making. J Mant Organ. 2010; 16: 382-398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Windschitl PD. Judging the accuracy of a likelihood judgment: The case of smoking risk. J Behav Decis Mak. 2002; 15: 19-35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Epstein S, Pacini R, Denes-Raj V, Heier H. Individual differences in intuitive–experiential and analytical–rational thinking styles. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996; 71: 390-405. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.390.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Weinstein ND, Kwitel A, McCaul KD, et al. Risk perceptions: Assessment and relationship to influenza vaccination. Health Psycholy. 2007; 26: 146-151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Mills B, Reyna VF, Estrada S. Explaining contradictory relations between risk perception and risk taking. Psychol Science. 2008; 19: 429-433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Fisher JD, Williams SS, Fisher WA, Malloy TE. Understanding AIDS risk behavior among sexually active urban adolescents: An empirical test of the information–motivation–behavioral skills model. AIDS Behav. 1999; 13: 13-23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Janssen E, van Osch L, Lechner L, Candel M, de Vries H. Thinking versus feeling: differentiating between cognitive and affective components of perceived cancer risk. Psychol Health. 2012; 27: 767-783.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Gerrard M, Gibbons FX, Houlihan AE, Stock ML, Pomery EA. A dual-process approach to health risk decision making: The prototype willingness model. Dev Rev. 2008; 28: 29-61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Portnoy DB, Ferrer RA, Bergman HE, Klein WM: Changing deliberative and affective responses to health risk: A meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev. 2013, ePub ahead of print.

  33. Janssen E, van Osch L, de Vries H, Lechner L. Measuring risk perceptions of skin cancer: Reliability and validity of different operationalizations. Brit J Health Psychol. 2011; 16: 92-112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Janssen E, Waters EA, van Osch L, Lechner L, de Vries H. The importance of affectively-laden beliefs about health risks: the case of tobacco use and sun protection. J Behav Med. 2014; 37: 11-21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Klein WMP, Harris PR, Ferrer RA, Zajac LE. Feelings of vulnerability in response to threatening messages: Effects of self-affirmation. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2011; 46: 1237-1242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Crites SL, Fabrigar LR, Petty RE. Measuring the affective and cognitive properties of attitudes: Conceptual and methodological issues. Persy Soc Psychol Bull. 1994; 20: 619-634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Keer M, van den Putte B, Neijens P. The role of affect and cognition in health decision making. Brit J Soc Psychol. 2010; 49: 143-153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Meyers-Levy J, Malaviya P. Consumers’ processing of persuasive advertisements: An integrative framework of persuasion theories. J Mark. 1999; 63: 45-60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Tramifow D, Sheeran P. Some tests of the distinction between cognitive and affective beliefs. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1998; 34: 378-397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. van den Berg H, Manstead ASR, van der Pligt J. J. WDH: The role of affect in attitudes toward organ donation and donor-relevant decisions. Psychol Health. 2005; 20: 789-802.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Peer E, Vosgerau J, Acquisti A: Reputation as a sufficient condition for data quality on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Behav Res Methods. 2-14, ePub ahead of print.

  42. Hoyert DL, Xu J: Deaths: Preliminary data for 2011. National Vital Statistics Report. Preliminary report and tables. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nvsr/nvsr61/nvsr61_06.pdf. 2012.

  43. Allahbakhsh M, Benatallah B: Quality Control in Crowdsourcing Systems. Retrieved Retrieved January 27, 2015 from http://hydra.infosys.tuwien.ac.at/Staff/sd/papers/Zeitschriftenartikel%20-%20Quality%20Control%20SD%202013.pdf,

  44. Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011; 6: 3-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Paolacci G, Chandler J. Inside the Turk understanding Mechanical Turk as a participant pool. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014; 23: 184-188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. HINTS: Health Information National Trends Survey database, National Cancer Institute. Retrieved Retrieved from hints.cancer.gov, November 17, 2014,

  47. Sheeran P, Orbell S. How confidently can we infer health beliefs from questionnaire responses? Psychol Health. 1996; 11: 273-290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Appel M, Gnambs T, Maio GR. A short measure of the need for affect. J Pers Assess. 2012; 94(4): 418-426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. McEachan R, Conner M, Taylor N, Lawton RJ: Prospective prediction of health-related behaviours with the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analysis supplemental tables. 2011.

  50. Sheeran P. Intention—behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review. EurJ Soc Psychol. 2002; 12: 1-36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Webb TL, Sheeran P. Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychol Bull. 2006; 132: 249.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Bulmer MG. Principles of Statistics. New York: Dover; 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Stoeber J, Kobori O, Brown A. Examining mutual suppression effects in the assessment of perfectionism cognitions: Evidence supporting multidimensional assessment. Assessment. 2014; 21(6): 647-660.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods. 2008; 40: 879-891.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Rimal RN, Juon HS. Use of the risk perception attitude framework for promoting breast cancer prevention. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2010; 40: 287-310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Ajzen I, Brown TC, Carvajal F. Explaining the discrepancy between intentions and actions: The case of hypothetical bias in contingent valuation. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2004; 30: 1108-1122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Waters EA, Hay JL, Orom H, Kiviniemi MT, Drake BF. “Don’t know” responses to risk perception measures implications for underserved populations. Med Decis. 2013; 33: 271-281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Schwartz MD, Taylor KL, Willard KS. Prospective association between distress and mammography utilization among women with a family history of breast cancer. J Behav Med. 2003; 26: 105-117.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Janis IL. Effects of fear arousal on attitude change: Recent developments in theory and research. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 1967; 3: 166-224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Weinstein ND. Misleading tests of health behavior theories. Ann Behav Med. 2007; 33: 1-10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Ferrer RA, Klein WMP. Risk perceptions and health behavior. Curr Opin Psychol. 2015, ePub ahead of print.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rebecca A. Ferrer PhD.

Ethics declarations

Conflicts of Interest

Authors’ Statement of Conflict of Interest and Adherence to Ethical Standards Rebecca A Ferrer, William MP Klein, Alexander Persoskie, Aya Avishai-Yitshak, and Paschal Sheeran declare that they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose. Research involved human participants; all research procedures were approved by the National Cancer Institute Special Studies IRB and/or University of North Carolina IRB. All studies involved informed consent.

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ferrer, R.A., Klein, W.M.P., Persoskie, A. et al. The Tripartite Model of Risk Perception (TRIRISK): Distinguishing Deliberative, Affective, and Experiential Components of Perceived Risk. ann. behav. med. 50, 653–663 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9790-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9790-z

Keywords

Navigation