Log in

Does Group discussion exaggerate or diminish males’ reactions to a sexual assault scene?

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This research examines how group discussion may influence males’ inclinations toward sexual assault. This experiment tested two competing explanations of how reactions toward a sexual assault scene are affected by group discussion. Group polarization predicts that strong or weak inclinations toward sexual assault among group members become exaggerated with group discussion. Accordingly, because unfavorable opinions of sexual assault are most prevalent, then discussion exaggerates this tendency such that group members’ reactions toward sexual assault will be even more unfavorable for sexual assault after group discussion. Alternatively, release theory proposes that group interaction allows members to follow their impulses by disinhibiting social constraints such that group discussion would exaggerate inclinations toward sexual assault among group members. The results of an experiment in which participants watched a video depiction involving a sexual assault and then gave responses related to similar situations showed a pattern of results providing support for group polarization and no support for release theory. Therefore, group discussion can be a positive social influence on tendencies toward anti-social behavior if those inclinations are not widespread among group members. This finding can inform prevention programs aimed at reducing permissive attitudes toward sexual violence and sexual assault occurrences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of data and material

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available in the Open Science Foundation repository, osf.io/ck3br.

Code availability:

Not applicable

Notes

  1. This research included multiple dependent variables which were selected based on an examination of the research literature that investigates reactions and responses to sexual assault or rape. Much of this research involves responses to depictions of a rape scene, often with a vignette (e.g., McCaul et al., 1990). Several of the measures in this experiment derive from established measures (e.g., state affect, Byrne et al., 1974; empathy with the victim, Fernandez & Marshall 2003). For other measures, items were constructed to directly assess the concept. For example, the attributions of responsibility were based on a structure informed by Shaver’s (1985) work on attribution. Consequently, the dependent variables selected allowed tests of the release theory and group polarization hypotheses while also being implicated as important reactions to sexual assault in previous work (e.g., intentions; Malamuth, 1989).

  2. Files with the set of instructions, details of the questionnaire items, and the SPSS data set are available at: osf.io/ck3br.

References

  • 15 Face Charges in Hazing Incident (2003). Retrieved August 9, 2021 from https://www.cbsnews.com/news/15-face-charges-in-hazing-incident/

  • Black, M. C., Basile, K. C., Breiding, M. J., Smith, S. G., Walters, M. L., Merrick, M. T., Chen, J., & Stevens, M. R. (2011). The National Intimate Partner and Sexual Violence Survey (NISVS): 2010 Summary Report. Atlanta, GA: National Center for Injury Prevention and Control, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, D., Fisher, J. D., Lamberth, J., & Mitchell, H. E. (1974). Evaluation of erotica: Facts or feelings? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 111–116. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035731

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coker, A. L., Cook-Craig, P. G., Williams, C. M., Fisher, B. S., Clear, E. R., Garcia, L. S., & Hegge, L. M. (2011). Evaluation of Green Dot: An active bystander intervention to reduce sexual violence on college campuses. Violence Against Women, 17(6), 777–796.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L., & Furby, L. (1970). How we should measure ‘change’: Or should we? Psychological Bulletin, 74, 68–80. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0029382

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • da Silva, T., Harkins, L., & Woodhams, J. (2013). Multiple perpetrator rape: An international phenomenon. In M. A. H. Horvath, & J. Woodhams (Eds.), Handbook of the study of multiple perpetrator rape (pp. 10–36). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, J. H., & Hinsz, V. B. (1982). Current research problems in group performance and group dynamics. In H. Brandstätter, J. H. Davis, & G. Stocker-Kreichgauer (Eds.), Group decision making (pp. 1–20). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, J. H., Spitzer, C. E., Nagao, D., & Stasser, G. T. (1978). Bias in social decisions by individuals and groups: An example from mock juries. In H. Brandstatter, J. H. Davis, & H. Schuler (Eds.), Dynamics of group decisions (pp. 33–52). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeGrassi, S. W., Morgan, W. B., Walker, S. S., Wang, Y., & Sabat, I. (2012). Ethical decision making: Group diversity holds the key. Journal of Leadership Accountability and Ethics, 9(6), 51–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, J. R. (2001). Ten difference score myths. Organizational Research Methods, 4, 265–287. https://doi.org/10.1177/109442810143005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, S. R., Bradshaw, K. A., & Hinsz, V. B. (2014). Denying rape but endorsing forceful intercourse: Exploring differences among responders. Violence and Gender, 1, 188–193. https://doi.org/10.1089/vio.2014.0022

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, S. R., & Hinsz, V. B. (2013). Exploring attitudinal variables predictive of how men perceive rape. Problems of Psychology in the 21st Century, 7, 16–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez, Y. M., & Marshall, W. L. (2003). Victim empathy, social self-esteem, and psychopathy in rapists. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 15, 11–26. https://doi.org/10.1177/107906320301500102

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hinsz, V. B., & Davis, J. H. (1984). Persuasive arguments theory, group polarization and choice shifts. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 260–268. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167284102012

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinsz, V. B., Tindale, R. S., & Vollrath, D. A. (1997). The emerging conceptualization of groups as information processors. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 43–64. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.43

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jaffe, S., (Producer), Lansing, S., Producer, Kaplan, J., & Director (1998). The Accused [Motion Picture]. United States: Paramount.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, D. A., & LaVoie, L. (1985). Separating individual and group effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 339–348. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.48.2.339

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, N., & Epley, N. (2015). Group discussion improves lie detection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(24), 7460–7465.

  • Lewin, K. (1947). Group decision and social change. In T. Newcomb, & E. Hartley (Eds.), Readings in Social Psychology (pp. 197–211). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loh, C., Gidycz, C. A., Lobo, T. R., & Luthra, R. (2005). A prospective analysis of sexual assault perpetration: Risk factors related to perpetrator characteristics. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20(10), 1325–1348. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260505278528

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malamuth, N. M. (1989). The attraction to sexual aggression scale: Part one. The Journal of Sex Research, 26, 26–49. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224498909551491

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCaul, K. D., Veltum, L. G., Boychecko, V., & Crawford, J. J. (1990). Understanding attributions attributions of victim blame for rape: Sex, violence and foreseeability. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 20, 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1990.tb00375.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGrath, J. E., Martin, J., & Kulka, R. A. (1982). Judgment calls in research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, D. G., & Lamm, H. (1975). The polarizing effect of group discussion. American Scientist, 63(3), 297–303.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, D. G., & Lamm, H. (1976). The group polarization phenomenon. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 602–627. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.83

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Sexual Violence Resource Center (2015). Info and stats for journalists: Statistics about sexual violence. Retrieved June 17, 2019 from https://assets.documentcloud.org/documents/4901665/Publications-Nsvrc-Factsheet-Media-Packet.pdf

  • Park, E. S., & Hinsz, V. B. (2015). Group interaction sustains positive moods and diminishes negative moods. Group Dynamics, 19, 290–298. https://doi.org/10.1037/gdn0000034

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Postmes, T., & Spears, R. (1998). Deindividuation and antinormative behavior: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 238–259. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.123.3.238

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pruitt, D. G. (1971). Choice shifts in group discussion: An introductory review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 20(3), 339–360. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031922

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sadler, M. S., & Judd, C. M. (2002). Overcoming dependent data: A guide to the analysis of group data. In M. A. Hogg, & R. S. Tindale (Eds.), Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Group Processes (pp. 497–524). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samuelson, C. D. (2022). Why were the police attacked on January 6th? Emergent norms, focus theory, and the invisibility of expectations. Group Dynamics, 26(3).

  • Semanko, A. M., & Hinsz, V. B. (2022). Getting by with a little help from bystanders: Group versus individual hel** in the presence of alcohol primes. North Dakota State University, Fargo, ND: Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaver, K. G. (1985). The attribution of blame: Causality, responsibility, and blameworthiness. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tesser, A. (1978). Self-generated attitude change. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 11, 289–338. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60010-6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Bommel, M., van Prooijen, J. W., Elffers, H., & Van Lange, P. A. (2016). Booze, bars, and bystander behavior: People who consumed alcohol help faster in the presence of others. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00128

  • Woodhams, J., Taylor, P. J., & Cooke, C. (2020). Multiple perpetrator rape: Is perpetrator violence the result of victim resistance, deindividuation, or leader–follower dynamics? Psychology of Violence, 10, 120–129. https://doi.org/10.1037/vio0000255

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zumbo, B. D. (1999). The simple difference score as an inherently poor measure of change: Some reality, much mythology. In B. Thompson (Ed.), Advances in Social Science Methodology (Vol. 5, pp. 269–304). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Jared Ladbury who served as the experimenter for this study. Anna Semanko is now at the College of St. Scholastica, Duluth, MN. Alexis Charles is now at the University of Missouri-St. Louis. Direct queries to Verlin B. Hinsz, NDSU Psych Dept 2765, P.O. Box 6050, Fargo, ND 58108 − 6050 or email Verlin.Hinsz@NDSU.edu.

Funding

No funding was received for conducting this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The study conception, design, material preparation, data collection and analysis were performed by Sarah R. Edwards and Verlin B. Hinsz. The first draft of the manuscript was written by Sarah R. Edwards and all authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Verlin B. Hinsz.

Ethics declarations

Conflicts of interest/Competing interests

The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.

Ethics approval

(include appropriate approvals or waivers): This research was approved by the North Dakota State University Institutional Review Board before data collection began.

Consent to participate

No identifying information was gathered from participants and all participants provided a signed informed consent to participate in this research.

Consent for publication:

Not Applicable.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hinsz, V.B., Edwards, S.R., Semanko, A.M. et al. Does Group discussion exaggerate or diminish males’ reactions to a sexual assault scene?. Curr Psychol 42, 24732–24741 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03397-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03397-0

Keywords

Navigation