Log in

Aesthetic evaluation of Chinese calligraphy: a cross-cultural comparative study

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Investigations of non-Western art forms and non-Western subjects are needed to understand cross-cultural empirical aesthetics. This study examined the aesthetic evaluation of Chinese calligraphy, a distinguished visual art in East Asia, through people’s preferences among five scripts. These scripts are representative of major artistic writing styles of Chinese characters. We conducted an experiment with Chinese (mean age = 22.64, SD = 2.90) and non-Chinese participants (International students from Asia, Africa, and Northern America; mean age = 26.76, SD = 2.35) and investigated through subjective measures and eye-tracking measures. We adopted Pleasure-Interest Model of Aesthetic Liking (PIA Model) and measured fluency processing, affective feelings, motivational evaluation, and overall likeness as subjective measures to capture the aesthetic evaluation. The two groups evaluated the scripts differently and showed distinctive preferences for scripts. Chinese participants perceived the stimuli to be more fluent than non-Chinese participants. Chinese rated cursive script most fluent, pleasant, interesting, and liked it most, whereas non-Chinese liked the official script the most and rated it most interesting. Regarding eye movement, Chinese participants had fewer fixations and shorter total fixation durations, relative to non-Chinese participants. Non-Chinese participants were more sensitive to the features of the scripts compared with Chinese participants. We suggest that culture could affect aesthetic evaluation of Chinese calligraphy. Experience of Chinese characters and aesthetic expectations of different cultures may contribute to the evaluation differences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

We promise that the data is available when required and permission to reproduce material from other sources.

The study has been reviewed and approved by School Ethnic Board.

Notes

  1. Five non-Chinese participants had learned a few Chinese characters. Eight participants reported recognition of one or more characters, including: “如” (by five participants), “门” (by three participants), and “食”, “字”, “风”, and “深” (by one or two participants).

References

  • Augustin, D., & Leder, H. (2006). Art expertise: A study of concepts and conceptual spaces. Psychology Science, 48(2), 135–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumgarten, A. (1954). Reflections on Poetry (K. Aschenbrenner & WB Holther, Trans.). University of California Press. (Original work published 1735).

  • Blijlevens, J., Carbon, C. C., Mugge, R., & Schoormans, J. P. (2012). Aesthetic appraisal of product designs: Independent effects of typicality and arousal. British Journal of Psychology, 103(1), 44–57. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02038.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, R. F., & D’Agostino, P. R. (1994). The attribution and discounting of perceptual fluency: Preliminary tests of a perceptual fluency/attributional model of the mere exposure effect. Social Cognition, 12(2), 103–128. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.1994.12.2.103

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brieber, D., Nadal, M., Leder, H., & Rosenberg, R. (2014). Art in time and space: Context modulates the relation between art experience and viewing time. PLoS ONE, 9(6), e99019. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0099019

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Britannica, T. Editors of Encyclopaedia (2007). Lishu. Encyclopedia Britannica. https://www.britannica.com/art/lishu. Accessed 16 June 2021

  • Britannica, T. Editors of Encyclopaedia (2017). DazhuanEncyclopedia Britannicahttps://www.britannica.com/topic/dazhuan. Accessed 16 June 2021

  • Britannica, T. Editors of Encyclopaedia (2019a). Caoshu. Encyclopedia Britannicahttps://www.britannica.com/topic/caoshu. Accessed 16 June 2021

  • Britannica, T. Editors of Encyclopaedia (2019b). Kaishu. Encyclopedia Britannica. https://www.britannica.com/art/kaishu. Accessed 16 June 2021

  • Britannica, T. Editors of Encyclopaedia (2019c). **ngshu. Encyclopedia Britannica. https://www.britannica.com/topic/xingshu. Accessed 16 June 2021

  • Che, J., Sun, X., Gallardo, V., & Nadal, M. (2018). Cross-cultural empirical aesthetics. Progress in Brain Research, 237, 77–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, T. (2011). Chinese Calligraphy. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chua, H. F., Boland, J. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (2005). Cultural variation in eye movements during scene perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(35), 12629–12633. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0506162102

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coursaris, C. K., Swierenga, S. J., & Watrall, E. (2008). An empirical investigation of color temperature and gender effects on web aesthetics. Journal of Usability Studies, 3(3), 103–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A. (2012). Native listening: Language experience and the recognition of spoken words. MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Di, B., Luo, M., Shi, K., Liu, C., & Jiao, Y. (2021). Fractal analyses reveal the origin of aesthetics in Chinese calligraphy. Contemporary Social Science, 2(3), 13–19. https://doi.org/10.19873/j.cnki.2096-0212.2021.02.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duchowski, A. T. (2017). Serious Gaze [Keynote address]. In 2017 9th International Conference on Virtual Worlds and Games for Serious Applications (VS-Games), Athens, Greece. https://doi.org/10.1109/VS-GAMES.2017.8056614

  • Ericsson, K. A. (2006). The Influence of Experience and Deliberate Practice on the Development of Superior Expert Performance. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance (pp. 683–703). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511816796.038

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fechner, G. T. (1876). Preschool of aesthetics. Breitkopf & Härtel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster, M., Leder, H., & Ansorge, U. (2013). It felt fluent, and I liked it: Subjective feeling of fluency rather than objective fluency determines liking. Emotion, 13(2), 280–289. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030115

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graf, L. K., & Landwehr, J. R. (2015). A dual-process perspective on fluency-based aesthetics: The pleasure-interest model of aesthetic liking. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 19(4), 395–410. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868315574978

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • He, L. (2022). Calligraphy creation and aesthetic interest. Chinese Calligraphy,(1), 5.(何来胜. (2022). 书法创作与审美趣味. **书法(1), 5.)

  • Huang, Y. (2019). Method and value: Methodology in the study of calligraphic aesthetics in the twentieth century. Chinese Calligraphy, 15(3), 189–191. (黄映恺. (2019). 方法与价值:二十世纪书法美学研究中的方法论. **书法(15), 3.189-191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ip, M. H. K., & Cutler, A. (2020). Universals of listening: Equivalent prosodic entrainment in tone and non-tone languages. Cognition, 202, 104311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104311

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, N., Margulis, E. H., Clayton, M., Hannon, E., Honing, H., Iversen, J., … & Wald-Fuhrmann, M. (2020). Cross-cultural work in music cognition: Challenges, insights, and recommendations. Music Perception37(3), 185–195.

  • Ji, L.-J., Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Culture, control, and perception of relationships in the environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5), 943–955. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.943

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kant, I., & Smith, N. K. (1929). Critique of pure reason. Bedford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, A., Sommer, L., Klöckner, C. A., & Hanss, D. (2020). Contextualizing information enhances the experience of environmental art. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 14(3), 264–275.

  • Kirchner, H., & Thorpe, S. J. (2006). Ultra-rapid object detection with saccadic eye movements: Visual processing speed revisited. Vision Research, 46(11), 1762–1776. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2005.10.002

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kostyk, A., & Huhmann, B. A. (2021). Perfect social media image posts: Symmetry and contrast influence consumer response. European Journal of Marketing, 55(6), 1747–1779. https://doi.org/10.1108/EJM-09-2018-0629

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landwehr, J. R., Labroo, A. A., & Herrmann, A. (2011). Gut liking for the ordinary: Incorporating design fluency improves automobile sales forecasts. Marketing Science, 30(3), 416–429. https://doi.org/10.1287/mksc.1110.0633

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landwehr, J. R., Wentzel, D., & Herrmann, A. (2013). Product design for the long run: Consumer responses to typical and atypical designs at different stages of exposure. Journal of Marketing, 77(5), 92–107. https://doi.org/10.1509/1547-7185-77.5.92

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, D. (2018). Lawless ness in my calligraphy - Zen spirit in the calligraphic creation of Su Shi. Chinese Calligraphy, 326(6), 142–145. 林丹慧. (2018). 我书意造本无法——苏轼书法创作中的禅宗精神. **书法, 326(6), 142-145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Y., Liang N. Y., Wang, D. J., Zhang, S. Y. (1990). **an Dai Han Yu Chang Yong Zi Dian [Dictionary of Modern Chinese words in common uses] (Yang, T. Y., Jie, C. Y., & Sun, W., Eds.). Yuhang Publishing House.

  • Locher, P. J. (2006). The usefulness of eye movement recordings to subject an aesthetic episode with visual art to empirical scrutiny. Psychology Science, 48(2), 106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locher, P., Gray, S., & Nodine, C. (1996). The structural framework of pictorial balance. Perception, 25(12), 1419–1436. https://doi.org/10.1068/p251419

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Locher, P., Krupinski, E. A., Mello-Thoms, C., & Nodine, C. F. (2007). Visual interest in pictorial art during an aesthetic experience. Spatial Vision, 21(1–2), 55–77. https://doi.org/10.1163/156856807782753868

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Locher, P. J. (2012). Empirical investigation of an aesthetic experience with art. Aesthetic Science: Connecting Minds, Brains, and Experience (A. P. Shimamura & S. E. Palmer, Eds.). Oxford University Press.

  • Locher, P. (2020). The study of eye movements in empirical aesthetics. In The Oxford Handbook of Empirical Aesthetics. Edited by Marcos Nadal and Oshin Vartanian. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198824350.013.12

  • Lu, N. (2004). The rhythm of ancient Chinese calligraphy. Academic Exchange, 8(4), 134–137. https://doi.org/10.3969/j.issn.1000-8284.2004.08.033 鲁宁.(2004).**古代书法的韵律. 学术交流(8), 4, 134-137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 922–934. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.922

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Masuda, T., Gonzalez, R., Kwan, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (2008). Culture and aesthetic preference: Comparing the attention to context of East Asians and Americans. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(9), 1260–1275. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208320555

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, S., & Landwehr, J. R. (2018). Quantifying visual aesthetics based on processing fluency theory: Four algorithmic measures for antecedents of aesthetic preferences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 12(4), 399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mikuni, J., Specker, E., Pelowski, M., Leder, H., & Kawabata, H. (2021). Is there a general ‘art fatigue’ effect? A cross-paradigm, cross-cultural study of repeated art viewing in the laboratory. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 16(2), 343–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitrovic, A., Hegelmaier, L. M., Leder, H., & Pelowski, M. (2020). Does beauty capture the eye, even if it’s not (overtly) adaptive? A comparative eye-tracking study of spontaneous attention and visual preference with VAST abstract art. Acta Psychologica, 209, 103–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103133

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, S. J., & Demorest, S. M. (2009). Cultural constraints on music perception and cognition. Progress in Brain Research, 178, 67–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • QuianQuiroga, R., & Pedreira, C. (2011). How do we see art: An eye-tracker study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 98. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00098

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, R. (2002). Reasons for the preference for symmetry. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(3), 415–416. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X02350076

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, R., Winkielman, P., & Schwarz, N. (1998). Effects of perceptual fluency on affective judgments. Psychological Science, 9(1), 45–48. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00008

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004a). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(4), 364–382. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_3

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, R., Wurtz, P., & Zimmermann, T. D. (2004b). Exploring “fringe” consciousness: The subjective experience of perceptual fluency and its objective bases. Consciousness and Cognition, 13(1), 47–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00049-7

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D. L., & Rayner, K. (1998). Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review, 105(1), 125–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Segall, M. H., Campbell, D. T., & Herskovits, M. J. (1966). The influence of culture on visual perception. Bobbs-Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shi, X. (2019). Chinese Calligraphy as “Force-Form.” Journal of Aesthetic Education, 53(3), 54–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, L. F., & Smith, J. K. (2006). The nature and growth of aesthetic fluency. In P. Locher, C. Martindale, & L. Dorfman (Eds.), New directions in aesthetics, creativity and the arts (pp. 47–58). Baywood Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szubielska, M., Imbir, K., & Szymańska, A. (2021). The influence of the physical context and knowledge of artworks on the aesthetic experience of interactive installations. Current Psychology, 40(8), 3702–3715. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00322-w

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tseng, Y., & Zeng, Y. (1993). A history of Chinese calligraphy. Chinese University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varnum, M. E., Grossmann, I., Kitayama, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2010). The origin of cultural differences in cognition: The social orientation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 9–13.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vissers, N., & Wagemans, J. (2021). Processing fluency, processing style, and aesthetic response to artistic photographs. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000388

  • Wassiliwizky, E., & Menninghaus, W. (2021). Why and how should cognitive science care about aesthetics? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(6), 437–449.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, S. J. (2008). Eye movement control during reading: Effects of word frequency and orthographic familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 34(1), 205–223. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.34.1.205

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wölfflin, H. (1994). Prolegomena zu einer Psychologie der Architektur [Pro- legomena to a psychology of architecture]. In H. F. Mallgrave & E. Ikonomou (Eds.), Empathy, form, and space: Problems in German aesthetics, 1873–1893 (pp. 149–190). Getty Center for the History of Art and the Humanities. (Original work published 1886)

  • Wurtz, P., Reber, R., & Zimmermann, T. D. (2008). The feeling of fluent perception: A single experience from multiple asynchronous sources. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(1), 171–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.07.001

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhan, D. (2018). The temporality of calligraphy and its formal aesthetic significance. Chinese Calligraphy, 5(3), 189–191. 詹冬华. (2018). 书法的时空性及其形式美学意义. **书法(5), 3. 189-191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, L., Atari, M., Schwarz, N., Newman, E. J., & Afhami, R. (2022b). Conceptual metaphors, processing fluency, and aesthetic preference. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 98, 104247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, W., Tao, Y., Lai, S., Zhao, X., Lai, S., & He, X. (2022a). Positive referential meaning and color metaphor bring beauty: Evidence on aesthetic appraisal of ancient Chinese character from Han, Bai, and Yi ethnic groups. Current Psychology, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-02728-5

  • Zhou, B., & Yan, Y. (2018). International communication and practice system of Chinese calligraphy. Chinese Calligraphy, 3(2), 200–201. 周斌&颜以琳.(2018).书法的国际传播与实践体系.**书法3(2), 200-201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, Y. (2018). Aesthetic value of calligraphy appreciation. China Literature and Art Criticism, 8(1), 77–85. 朱以撒. (2018). 书法欣赏中的审美价值. **文艺评论(8), 9. 77-85.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China [71902113].

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yi Xu.

Ethics declarations

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual adult participants included in the study; The authors promise that the data is available when required and will make our data available in an open repository as required.

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix 1 Character stimulus

Table 4

Table 4 Details of 10 selected characters

Appendix 2 Repeated measures multiple analysis of variance for subjective measures

Table 5

Table 5 Repeated measures multiple analysis of variance for subjective measures

Appendix 3 Repeated Measures Multiple Analysis of Variance for Eye Movement Measures

Table 6

Table 6 Repeated measures multiple analysis of variance for eye movement measures

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Xu, Y., Shen, R. Aesthetic evaluation of Chinese calligraphy: a cross-cultural comparative study. Curr Psychol 42, 23096–23109 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03390-7

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03390-7

Keywords

Navigation