Money is a major source of family conflict, and money-related conflicts in couple relationships are rated by partners as more reoccurring, important, and problematic than non-money conflicts (Papp et al., 2009, 2018). Moreover, money-related conflicts between two partners harm their relational well-being (e.g., diminished marital satisfaction and increased likelihood of divorce; Britt & Huston, 2012; Dew et al., 2012). Despite the lack of studies that empirically investigated these associations, researchers have hinted at the possibility of money-related conflicts between two partners being associated with the two partners’ offspring’s money management and, in turn, well-being (Allen et al., 2007; Hancock et al., 2013). Specifically, individuals may learn money management by observing how other family members (e.g., their parents) interact about money (Gudmunson & Danes, 2011; LeBaron & Kelley, 2021). Besides, exposure to interparental financial conflicts—money-related conflicts or disagreements between parents—while growing up can be emotionally charged for the offspring (Ramzan et al., 2021). Emotionally charged money-related experiences can influence beliefs and behavioral patterns related to money management in offspring; these patterns may become deeply ingrained and persist into adulthood, ultimately affecting their well-being (Britt et al., 2015; Klontz & Britt, 2012; Klontz et al., 2015).

Taking the first step to investigate associations among interparental financial conflicts, financial beliefs and behaviors, and well-being among young adults, we aim to contribute to the existing literature as follows. First, we applied family systems theory to personal and family finance by investigating how interparental conflicts specific to money issues can spill over into associations with offspring’s development (Britt, 2016). Second, we also expanded the scope of family financial socialization theory by responding to the call to investigate the role of family interactions and relationships as an understudied component of financial socialization processes (Allsop et al., 2020; Hancock et al., 2013; Lanz et al., 2020).

Figure 1 displays the conceptual model. For indicators of interparental financial conflicts, we included two facets: (1) frequency, or how often parents disagree with each other, not characterized as any specific form of behavior; (2) resolution strategies, or actions that parents take to manage conflicts (for a review, see van Eldik et al., 2020). Resolution strategies during interparental financial conflicts have been commonly regarded as multi-dimensional, with each strategy being uniquely associated with offspring’s development (van Eldik et al., 2020; Zhou & Buehler, 2017). Realizing that the resolution of family conflicts can be culturally determined (Ramzan et al., 2021), we measured not only negotiation and hostility strategies—which have been studied previously in the U.S. or other Western samples (Dew & Dakin, 2011; Dew et al., 2012; Gibby et al., 2021; Hancock et al., 2013)—but also the triangulation strategy. Triangulation refers to offspring being involved in interparental conflicts to modulate these conflicts (e.g., parents expecting the offspring to be on their side or to make the offspring act as the moderator; Kerig, 2016). The triangulation strategy seems relatively common in Chinese societies (including Hong Kong, the focal cultural context of the current study) because family members may feel obligated to devote time and efforts to maintain family harmony (Kwok et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2017). We included negotiation, hostility, and triangulation—along with frequency—in the same model to investigate how they operated above and beyond each other.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The conceptual model (N = 312). Note. The dark arrows indicate hypothesized positive associations, and the grey arrows indicate hypothesized negative associations in the present study section

We included money vigilance (a belief that money should be handled with heightened alertness and privacy; Britt et al., 2015; Klontz et al., 2015) and healthy money management (including spending within a budget and regular saving; Dew & ** review. Adolescent Research Review, 2(4), 255–292. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40894-016-0052-x " href="/article/10.1007/s10834-024-09972-w#ref-CR48" id="ref-link-section-d221335664e581">2017) and life satisfaction (i.e., the overall evaluation of how happy and satisfied young adults are with life; Diener et al., 1985). Healthy money management is robustly beneficial as young adults pursue economic independence and fulfill other important life goals, and should be associated with high levels of financial well-being and life satisfaction (Chan et al., 2021; Li et al., 2022; Sorgente & Lanz, 2017).

The possible associations between money vigilance and well-being (financial well-being and life satisfaction) are mixed. Whereas money-vigilant individuals—due to their caution with money—experience high financial well-being, their excessive concerns about money may prohibit them from enjoying the benefits and convenience that money brings to life (Britt et al., 2015; Klontz et al., 2015). Including these two constructs—money vigilance and healthy money management—and testing them as possible mediators between interparental financial conflicts and young adults’ financial well-being or life satisfaction could demonstrate mixed results of experiencing interparental financial conflicts for the offspring.

Theoretical Frameworks

We integrated the theories of family systems and family financial socialization as our guiding frameworks to examine the mediational model (see Fig. 1) of interparental financial conflicts to young adults’ well-being via young adults’ financial beliefs and behaviors. According to family systems theory, the dynamics between two parents and their offspring—as subsystems within the family—should be interrelated with each other, and problems in the parents’ romantic bonds can spill over into their offspring’s development (Kerig, 2016). Connecting family systems theory to our study, interparental financial conflicts represent parents’ relational problems (Papp et al., 2009, 2018), and young adults’ financial beliefs, financial behaviors, and well-being represent their offspring’s development (Li et al., 2019, 2022).

According to family financial socialization theory, the family is the earliest and most influential setting in which individuals learn money management (Gudmunson & Danes, 2011; LeBaron & Kelley, 2021). In addition to parental financial socialization—how parents teach their offspring about money—family interactions and relationships should also be associated with the younger generation’s formation and development of financial beliefs and behaviors (Gudmunson & Danes, 2011; LeBaron & Kelley, 2021). Specific to interparental financial conflicts, these types of conflicts can be considered as financial socialization (Gudmunson & Danes, 2011). For example, parents’ financial conflict could be a form of financial discussion as the offspring are brought into these interparental conflicts. Parents’ financial conflict could also be a form of parent financial modeling as parents set the example for managing and discussing money with others. Connecting family financial socialization theory to our study, the younger generation may learn financial norms and anticipate how they will handle money after observing or being caught in interparental financial conflicts. Further, according to family financial socialization theory, young adults’ financial beliefs and behaviors formed during their early years will ultimately be associated with the younger generation’s well-being (Gudmunson & Danes, 2011; LeBaron & Kelley, 2021).

Empirical Studies

Interparental Financial Conflicts: Frequency and Resolution Strategies

As with other types of conflicts between parents, interparental financial conflicts are a multi-faceted construct (van Eldik et al., 2020). Regarding the frequency of interparental financial conflicts, money-related disagreements between two partners (i.e., the two parents) took place in 80% of households, with the frequency as “sometimes” to “often” in 45% of households (Britt & Huston, 2012). Compared to non-money-related conflicts (e.g., housework, sex, and in-laws), disagreements over money were more frequent between the two partners (Dew & Dakin, 2011; Dew et al., 2012). Thus, it is common for offspring to be exposed to money-related conflicts between their parents (Ramzan et al., 2021).

Regarding the resolution strategies of interparental financial conflicts, prior researchers have usually focused on negotiation (the behaviors of facilitating the progress of resolution via calming discussion and problem-solving) and hostility (anger expression in verbally, non-verbally, and physically aggressive ways) (Dew & Dakin, 2011; Dew et al., 2012; Gibby et al., 2021; Hancock et al., 2013). The assessment of these two strategies—negotiation and hostility—was consistent with prior studies focusing on constructive and destructive ways to handle conflicts between parents (Kopystynska et al., 2020; van Eldik et al., 2020; Zhou et al., 2021). Whereas negotiation is a constructive resolution strategy, hostility is a destructive resolution strategy (Kopystynska et al., 2020; van Eldik et al., 2020; Zhou et al., 2021). Notably, as compared to non-money conflicts, money-related conflicts between the two partners were handled with higher levels of hostility and lower levels of negotiation, possibly because money is a taboo topic related to one’s self-evaluation and vulnerability (Shapiro, 2007) and may therefore provoke anger and self-defensiveness (Dew et al., 2012; Dew & Dakin, 2011; Papp et al., 2009, 2018).

In addition to negotiation and hostility, we also examine the strategy of triangulation, which has been studied as an interparental conflict strategy in the U.S. and other Western samples (Kerig, 1996, 2016). The high value placed on harmonious family relationships in Chinese societies necessitates our inclusion of triangulation as another resolution strategy for interparental financial conflicts (Kwok et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2017). Because family members may assume that the offspring is a responsible party in maintaining family relationships, the offspring may be caught in disagreements between parents (e.g., being forced to take sides, and finding a way to solve interparental conflicts; Wang et al., 2017). Triangulation is associated with many adverse outcomes for offspring (e.g., poor psychological well-being and hindered personal growth) and is regarded as destructive versus constructive in handling interparental financial conflicts (Kwok et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2017).

Interparental Financial Conflicts and Young Adults’ Financial Beliefs and Behaviors

Prior researchers have found preliminary evidence for associations between interparental financial conflicts and young adults’ financial beliefs and behaviors. The hostility strategy used during interparental financial conflicts is associated with young adults’ less healthy money management behaviors (e.g., unnecessary shop** with credit cards; Allen et al., 2007; Hancock et al., 2013). Regarding associations between interparental financial conflicts and money vigilance, Allen et al.’s study (2007) suggested the possibility that both frequency and the hostility strategy may be related to high levels of money vigilance because those who were repeatedly exposed to interparental arguments about money were more likely to value money as an avenue of achieving independence and view money with great caution (compared to young adults who witnessed parents collaborate on money issues while growing up).

Young Adults’ Financial Beliefs and Behaviors and Young Adults’ Well-Being

Associations between healthy money management and high levels of financial well-being or life satisfaction are well-established (for reviews, see Goyal & Kumar, 2021; Sorgente & Lanz, 2017; for empirical studies, see Chan et al., 2021; Dew & ** review. Adolescent Research Review, 2(4), 255–292. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40894-016-0052-x " href="/article/10.1007/s10834-024-09972-w#ref-CR48" id="ref-link-section-d221335664e855">2017). Regarding associations between interparental financial conflicts and young adults’ life satisfaction, studies on interparental conflicts more generally (versus interparental conflicts specific to money) are informative.

Specifically, interparental conflicts are repeatedly identified as predictors of offspring’s well-being in non-financial domains (e.g., life satisfaction, mental health, and personal growth; for reviews, see Cao et al., 2022; Tiwari & Verma, 2019; van Eldik et al., 2020). High conflict frequency and the use of hostility and triangulation strategies are associated with low levels of offspring’s well-being in non-financial domains (e.g., low levels of life satisfaction and high levels of depression and anxiety; Cao et al., 2022; Kwok et al., 2020; Tiwari & Verma, 2019; van Eldik et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2017). The use of a negotiation resolution strategy is associated with high levels of offspring’s well-being in non-financial domains (van Eldik et al., 2020; Zhou et al., 2021). As interparental conflicts on money issues are more frequent and are characterized by higher hostility and lower negotiation (compared to non-money related conflicts; Dew et al., 2012; Dew & Dakin, 2011; Papp et al., 2009, 2018), the frequency and resolution strategies of interparental financial conflicts should also be associated with young adults’ life satisfaction.

The Current Study

Using data collected from Hong Kong young adults, we aimed to investigate associations among interparental financial conflicts, young adults’ financial beliefs and behaviors, and young adults’ well-being. Integrating the theories of family systems and family financial socialization and empirical studies, we hypothesize the following.

Regarding the associations between interparental financial conflicts and financial beliefs or behaviors (H1): High frequency, hostility strategy, and triangulation strategy should be associated with high levels of money vigilance but low levels of healthy money management (H1a-H1c). Collaboration strategy should be associated with high levels of money vigilance and high levels of healthy money management (H1d).

Regarding the associations between interparental financial conflicts and young adults’ well-being (H2): High frequency, hostility strategy, and triangulation strategy should be associated with low levels of financial well-being and life satisfaction (H2a-H2c). Negotiation strategy should be associated with high levels of financial well-being and life satisfaction (H2d).

Regarding the associations between young adults’ financial beliefs and behaviors and young adults’ well-being (H3): High levels of money vigilance should be associated with high levels of financial well-being but low levels of life satisfaction (H3a). Healthy money management should be associated with high levels of financial well-being and high levels of life satisfaction (H3b).

Regarding indirect pathways (H4): The associations between interparental financial conflicts and young adults’ well-being (as stated in H2) should be mediated by the levels of money vigilance (H4a) and healthy money management (H4b).

Methods

Participants and Procedures

Data in this study were drawn from a larger project in which researchers aimed to understand finance and well-being among Hong Kong young adults (aged 18–30 years old). Materials and data from the larger project can be obtained by contacting the corresponding author. In the larger project, researchers collected the survey data in March and April of 2022 from 604 young adults via Qualtrics. IRB approval was obtained at the corresponding author’s home institution (project number anonymized for peer review). To be included in the larger project, young adults (1) had to have been living in Hong Kong for at least a year (so that participants will have had some experience and understanding of money management in Hong Kong), and (2) had to be able to read the Chinese language (so they could complete the survey, which was in the Chinese language). Researchers in the larger project used the quota sampling method and considered age and gender to recruit participants. As a result, the distribution of age and gender among 604 young adults was equivalent to the population of Hong Kong young adults. In addition, the 604 young adults were generally diverse in age, education, and income levels (see Supplementary Document 1 for details). The survey took up to 20–25 min. Upon completing the survey, each participant received 20 HKD as compensation.

Given the aim of the current study, we only included young adults who reported some incidence of interparental financial conflicts while growing up (i.e., indicating “yes” to the question “By the time you turned 18 years old, did your parents have any conflicts or disagreements on money?”). The final sample in this study included 312 Hong Kong young adults. Among 312 participants, 56.7% identified as female and 43.3% as male, and no other gender identity was reported. The median age was 24–26 years old; 16.7% were 18–20 years old, 26.3% were 21–23 years old, 24.7% were 24–26 years old, and 32.4% were 27–30 years old. For education, only 21 young adults (6.7%) were current students; the median degree was a bachelor’s degree; 22.4% had a high school degree or less, 17.9% had an associate degree, 51.9% had a bachelor’s degree, and 7.7% had a master’s or doctoral degree. For personal monthly income, 49.2% were below the local median, and the other 50.8% were above the local median (the median income in Hong Kong = about 20,000 HKD per month; Census & Statistics Department of Hong Kong SAR, 2023).

Measures

Most measures were originally developed in Western societies and published in English. To ensure the wording accuracy of the Chinese version, researchers in the larger project conducted translation and back translation (Brislin, 1986). The full measures (in English) are in Supplementary Document 2. The Chinese version can be obtained by contacting the corresponding author.

Interparental Financial Conflicts—Frequency and Resolution Strategies.

Among young adults who remembered being exposed to interparental financial conflicts while growing up, researchers in the larger project used a self-developed, ordinal item to assess the frequency of these conflicts (“How often did your parents have conflicts or disagreements around money?”). The responses were from 1 (rarely) to 4 (always). Among the 312 participants, the median was “sometimes;” 32.7% reported 1 (rarely), 41.3% reported 2 (sometimes), 12.5% reported 3(often), and 3.5% reported 4 (always).

Researchers in the larger project developed another item for resolution strategies: participants could select from a list all actions their parents took to manage money-related conflicts (“When your parents had conflicts or disagreements about money, they ____”). The list was developed based on prior studies on conflict resolution strategies between two parents (van Eldik et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2017), which included “negotiated with each other,” “blamed each other,” “complained about each other to you,” “had you pass information to each other or moderate,” and “cursed or beat up each other.”

For each behavior in the list, participants indicated “yes” or “no” (see Supplementary Document 3 for the frequency analyses of each behavior). In the current study, we followed prior studies (van Eldik et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2017) and created three binary variables to reflect the use of negotiation strategy (i.e., parents negotiated with each other), hostility strategy (i.e., parents blamed each other and/or cursed or beat up each other), and triangulation strategy (i.e., parents complained about each other to their offspring and/or parents had their offspring pass information to each other or moderate). For these binary variables, “0” indicated that parents did not use the stated strategy, and “1” indicated that parents used the stated strategy.

Money Vigilance

We measured vigilance using the 8-item money vigilance subscale from the Klontz Money Script Inventory-Revised (Taylor et al., 2016). Participants indicated their agreement with each statement on a six-point scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). An example item is “I would be a nervous wreck if I did not have money saved for an emergency.” For descriptive analyses, we calculated scale scores by averaging all items, and higher scores indicated higher levels of money vigilance. In the main analyses, we created a latent construct. The Cronbach’s alpha of this scale was 0.70. The omega reliability, calculated according to Hayes and Coutts (2020), was 0.66.

Healthy Money Management

We assessed healthy money management by adapting seven items from Dew and ** review. Adolescent Research Review, 2(4), 255–292. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40894-016-0052-x " href="/article/10.1007/s10834-024-09972-w#ref-CR48" id="ref-link-section-d221335664e1093">2017), we assessed financial well-being using three different scales: financial distress (three items; Prawitz et al., 2006), financial satisfaction (one item; ** the young adult heal from any adverse effects.

Limitations, Strengths, and Future Directions

Some limitations need to be noted. First, the study used cross-sectional data; thus, we could not determine causal relationships among the variables. Future researchers could use longitudinal designs to verify how IPFC may be associated with young adults’ financial attitudes and behaviors and, in turn, well-being. Second, the assessment of IPFC was based on how young adults (18–30 years old) remembered their experiences while growing up (before 18 years of age). Yet in Asia, parents may hide IPFC from their offspring, aiming to shield them from unnecessary worries (Ramzan et al., 2021). The sample size may therefore have been diminished, which limited the statistical power of our analyses. The frequency of IPFC and parents’ utilization of destructive resolution strategies (i.e., triangulation and hostility in the current study) may also have been underestimated.

Additionally, when investigating associations between young adults’ report of early experiences and their current well-being, the findings may have been further complicated by other constructs (e.g., the current relationship status of parents [still married versus divorced or separated or widowed], when those conflicts occurred [early childhood versus late childhood versus adolescence], and young adults’ experiences after turning 18 years old). In the sensitivity analyses, we identified little evidence for the confounding roles of the current relationship status of parents (see Supplementary Document 8 for details) or the differences between those who were younger and those who were older (18–23 years old versus 24–30 years; see Supplementary Document 9 for details). Information regarding when those conflicts occurred, unfortunately, was not collected, preventing us from additional sensitivity analyses. Collectively, for a more accurate estimation, future researchers may collect data from parents with offspring below the age of 18 and ask about parents’ and young adults’ current experiences. To obtain adequate statistical power, 580 households should be included (see Supplementary Document 10 for Monte Carlo power analysis).

Third, the utilization of single items—which was also the case in existing studies on the same topic (Allen et al., 2007; Gibby et al., 2021; Hancock et al., 2013)—may limit the reliability and validity of our assessment of IPFC frequency and resolution strategies. Further, using binary variables (yes versus no) to assess resolution strategies—as compared to using continuous measures—cannot reflect the extent that each strategy was used. Given the drawbacks of these single-item measures, our findings regarding IPFC are still relatively preliminary. Because a field is only as good as its measures and a well-established measure is a prerequisite for sound conclusions (LeBaron-Black et al., 2022), future researchers should develop and validate a new assessment tool for the frequency and resolution strategies of IPFC.

In terms of strengths, our sample was specific to Hong Kong young adults, a still understudied population. In family finance research, the focus on White young adults—of whom many are also college students and from the United States—has been noted in several reviews or commentaries (Curran et al., 2021; LeBaron & Kelley, 2021). Thus, we add to the literature in terms of what is known about family financial constructs as specific to Hong Kong young adults. We also encourage future researchers to continue to study young adults in understudied cultural contexts. Second, we drew from family systems theory and family financial socialization theory for our study constructs and to generate our hypotheses. Third, the examination of IPFC is novel in family finance research, including the measurement of frequency as well as the three types of strategies (i.e., negotiation, hostility, triangulation) that we assessed in the current study. Finally, as many young adults may have been exposed to IPFC during childhood and adolescence (Britt & Huston, 2012; Dew & Dakin, 2011; Dew et al., 2012), we suggest that future researchers and practitioners include IPFC as study constructs in their work on how to facilitate young adults’ financial management and well-being.

In sum, integrating family systems theory and family financial socialization theory, we examined associations among IPFC, financial beliefs and behaviors, and well-being among young adults in Hong Kong. Informed by family systems theory, we found that resolution strategies of IPFC–especially negotiation and hostility, and to a lesser extent, triangulation–can spill over into offspring’s financial beliefs and well-being. However, the frequency of IPFC was barely associated with young adults’ development in the financial domain. Drawing from family financial socialization theory, we evaluated the mediating roles played by financial beliefs and behaviors in associations between IPFC and well-being. Collectively, we extended the utilization of family systems theory in the field of family and personal finance. We also expanded the scope of family financial socialization theory by responding to the call for researchers to test how young adults learn money management from family interactions and relationships in a sample of Hong Kong young adults.