figure b

Introduction

The experience of hypoglycaemia is generally believed to affect people with diabetes in a negative manner [1, 2]. Thus, several retrospective studies demonstrate that self-reported hypoglycaemia is associated with poorer quality of life (QoL) in type 1 diabetes [1, 3,4,5], an effect amplified by the frequency and severity of the hypoglycaemic episodes [3, 5, 6]. Nocturnal experimental hypoglycaemia is shown to reduce QoL the following day [7]. A few survey studies compare the effect of nocturnal vs daytime hypoglycaemic events on QoL and find that people with self-reported nocturnal mild hypoglycaemia report poorer QoL compared with those reporting daytime hypoglycaemia [3, 5].

Also, mood may be affected by hypoglycaemia in type 1 diabetes. A prospective study shows that people with severe hypoglycaemia chronically have a higher level of anxiety and depression compared with people without severe hypoglycaemia but no acute effects of the episodes per se [8]. Other studies explore the consequences of symptomatic mild hypoglycaemia on mood in type 1 diabetes [9, 10] and find that mood shifts towards unhappiness and a less energetic and tenser state [9, 10].

Previous research mainly focuses on the consequences of symptomatic hypoglycaemia, which, on one hand, may be a product of biological effects of hypoglycaemia on the brain and, on the other hand, may be psychological effects arising from the participants’ knowledge of the episodes. Little is known about the impact of asymptomatic hypoglycaemia. Since the majority of hypoglycaemic episodes in type 1 diabetes are asymptomatic [11, 12], these episodes may potentially have a major impact on QoL, mood and effectiveness during daily life.

Studies applying advanced neuroimaging modalities to assess the effect of hypoglycaemia on brain function show that people with impaired awareness of hypoglycaemia may react differently to hypoglycaemia compared with people with normal hypoglycaemia awareness [13, 14], suggesting diverse biological effects of hypoglycaemia.

The aim of this study was to investigate the impact of preceding spontaneous nocturnal hypoglycaemia (asymptomatic, in particular) on QoL, mood and effectiveness during the following days in type 1 diabetes. Furthermore, we aimed to explore potential differences in QoL responses according to state of hypoglycaemia awareness.

Methods

The study was a prospective observational study approved by the Danish Data Protection Agency and the Regional Committee on Biomedical Research Ethics (H-15002715). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. A detailed description of the study design has been published previously [12].

Participants

A total of 153 adults (≥18 years old) with type 1 diabetes duration of at least 1 year were recruited consecutively in the outpatient clinic at Nordsjællands Hospital Hillerød, Denmark. Individuals with concomitant malignant disease or end-stage renal disease, or those who were pregnant, were excluded from the study. The baseline characteristics of the participants are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Characteristics of participants with type 1 diabetes

Study design

Following recording of baseline characteristics and blood sampling, the participants underwent 6–7 days of blinded continuous glucose monitoring (CGM), while they continued their usual daily activities and treatments. During the monitoring period, the participants documented episodes of symptomatic hypoglycaemia (≤3.9 mmol/l). Daily activities, food intake and insulin dosing were recorded every half hour in a diary. A detailed questionnaire was filled in by the participants regarding each episode of hypoglycaemia, including whether they had symptoms or not.

The device used for CGM was blinded iPro 2 with Enlite Glucose sensors (Medtronic MiniMed, Northridge, CA, USA), which has a reported overall mean absolute relative difference (MARD) of 13.9% and 18.4% in the hypoglycaemic range (glucose level ≤ 3.9 mmol/l), a sensitivity of 90.1% and a positive predictive value of 83.8% for hypoglycaemia [15]. Calibration was performed according to the manufacturer’s recommendations (four times daily) with blood glucose meter measurements (Contour XT meter using Contour NEXT test strips; Bayer, Basel, Switzerland).

Questionnaires

At entry into the study, the participants filled in questionnaires regarding self-estimated state of hypoglycaemia awareness according to three validated methods: Gold [16]; Clarke [17]; and Hillerød [18]. Furthermore, the questionnaire asked the participant to state the number of episodes of severe hypoglycaemia experienced in the preceding year. Severe hypoglycaemia was defined as an episode where assistance from another person was needed to restore glucose levels to normal [2]. History of diabetes, treatments (medications, use of technologies) and late diabetic complications were collected from the participants’ medical records or asked for as appropriate. HbA1c and C-peptide levels were measured (participants with undetectable levels (<20 pmol/l) were categorised as C-peptide negative).

Every evening participants filled in questionnaires regarding QoL, mood and self-estimated effectiveness at work. QoL was assessed by the WHO-5 Well-Being Index [19] and the EuroQol-5D visual analogue scale (EQ-5D VAS) [20]. In both questionnaires, the participants score between 0 and 100, with higher scores indicating higher QoL. A difference in score of ≥7 on the EQ-5D VAS is reported to be a clinically important change in QoL [21]. Changes in mood were evaluated by the University of Wales Institute of Science and Technology (UWIST) Mood Adjective Checklist [22]. This checklist describes three main mood states: energetic arousal (feeling active rather than tired); tense arousal (feeling anxious vs relaxed); and hedonic tone (feeling happy vs sad). Self-estimated effectiveness at work was assessed hourly using a Likert-type visual analogue scale of 1 (not effective) to 7 (very effective).

CGM analysis

An episode of hypoglycaemia was classified as an episode with continuous interstitial glucose values below two threshold levels: interstitial glucose ≤3.9 mmol/l (IG3.9), as formerly recommended by the ADA; and interstitial glucose ≤3.0 mmol/l (IG3.0), as suggested by the International Hypoglycaemia Study Group and endorsed by ADA and the EASD [2, 23,24,25]. A valid hypoglycaemic event was defined by glucose values below the thresholds for ≥15 min, and recovery from hypoglycaemia was defined as interstitial glucose values continuously above the threshold for ≥20 min [23, 24, 26]. An episode of hypoglycaemia without self-reported symptoms of hypoglycaemia in the diary was classified as asymptomatic. The hypoglycaemic nadir of an episode was defined as the lowest interstitial glucose value during the episode. Night-time was defined as 11:00 hours to 07:00 hours. To assess whether daytime and night-time hyperglycaemia had an effect on QoL, an episode of significant hyperglycaemia was defined as glucose levels continuously >14 mmol/l for ≥15 min [23, 24]. Time in range (TIR) was defined as the percentage of time spent with a glucose level 4–10 mmol/l [23, 24].

Statistical analysis

A total of 153 patients were included but QoL data were not reported for two patients and awareness status was unknown for one patient, so n ranged from 150 to 153 in analyses. This study was an explorative study, so no power calculations could be made. We used standard descriptive statistics to characterise participants. Median and range are presented in addition to mean ± SD, when distribution of data is skewed. The Mixed Models analysis was used to investigate the effect of prior nocturnal hypoglycaemia on QoL, mood and effectiveness the day after. The Mixed Models analysis enables adjustment for correlation due to repeated observations (i.e. 6 days of CGM) on each participant. For categorical variables, the resulting estimate (i.e. increase or decrease in scores on the questionnaires) and p values indicate comparison of nights without hypoglycaemia vs hypoglycaemic nights (reference: non-hypoglycaemic nights). For quantitative variables, the estimate and p values indicate the increase per increment of the variable. The following variables were included in the model: episode of hypoglycaemia the preceding night (present or not) and duration of hypoglycaemic episodes (per 15 min increment [i.e. the shortest duration of a hypoglycaemic episode]). All variables were assessed in total as well as separated into asymptomatic and symptomatic nocturnal hypoglycaemia. To assess the influence of possible confounders we also included the following variables in the model: TIR (daytime); episode of daytime hyperglycaemia (present or not); episode of nocturnal hyperglycaemia (present or not); fasting morning self-monitoring of blood glucose (SMBG), categorised into three groups (hypoglycaemic morning SMBG ≤3.9 mmol/l [SMBG≤3.9], morning SMBG in range [SMBG4–10; glucose levels of 4–10 mmol/l] and hyperglycaemic morning SMBG >10 mmol/l [SMBG>10]); and HbA1c. Non-parametric tests were used to compare daytime glycaemic variables on days with and without prior nocturnal hypoglycaemia.

Analyses were performed with SPSS software package (version 25.0; IBM, Armonk, NY, USA), and a p value <0.05 (two-sided) was chosen as level of statistical significance.

Results

Occurrence of nocturnal hypoglycaemia

The 153 participants were monitored for a total of 1040 days and nights, resulting in 6.1 ± 0.9 (mean ± SD) days and nights per person of valid recording of CGM (total duration minus periods of time with failure to record [0.5% of the total recording duration]). During the study, 54% of the participants had one or more nights with hypoglycaemia. There was at least one episode of nocturnal hypoglycaemia at threshold IG3.9 in 141 nights (14%), and at threshold IG3.0 in 83 nights (8%). Asymptomatic nocturnal hypoglycaemia occurred in 102 nights (10%) at threshold IG3.9 and in 60 nights (6%) at threshold IG3.0. Symptomatic nocturnal hypoglycaemic episodes occurred at IG3.9 in 40 nights (4%) and at IG3.0 in 24 nights (2%). The duration of nocturnal hypoglycaemic episodes (mean ± SD) was 181 ± 120 min (median [range], 150 [20–535] min) at IG3.9, and 147 ± 107 min (median [range], 125 [20–450] min) at IG3.0. The duration of asymptomatic hypoglycaemic episodes was 192 ± 122 min (median [range], 173 [20–535] min) at IG3.9, and 160 ± 109 min (median [range], 140 [20–450] min) at IG3.0. The duration of symptomatic episodes was 158 ± 119 min (median [range], 115 [20–465] min) at IG3.9, and 127 ± 117 min (median [range], 76 [20–450] min) and IG3.0.

QoL following nocturnal hypoglycaemia

EQ-5D VAS

The participants reported significantly higher self-estimated QoL, indicated by higher scores on the EQ-5D VAS, after hypoglycaemic nights as compared with after non-hypoglycaemic nights when analysed at both threshold IG3.9 and threshold IG3.0 (Table 2). The effect size on QoL increased with decreasing hypoglycaemic levels. However, hypoglycaemic nights with glucose measurements between 3.0 and 3.9 mmol/l did not have a significant effect on QoL the following day (score difference: 1.9 [95% CI –0.88, 4.6], p = 0.18). The effect on EQ-5D VAS increased with longer total duration of nocturnal hypoglycaemia (score increase per 15 min increment in hypoglycaemic duration: 0.14 [95% CI 0.01, 0.27], p = 0.032, for IG3.9; and 0.23 [95% CI 0.02, 0.44], p = 0.030, for IG3.0). However, the duration of episodes with glucose measurements between 3.0 and 3.9 mmol/l did not influence EQ-5D VAS (0.07 [95% CI −0.20, 0.34], p = 0.55). The effect of nocturnal hypoglycaemia on EQ-5D VAS was limited to the day following the event, as no association was present on the second day following the event (p = 0.59).

Table 2 QoL (assessed by the EQ-5D VAS and by the WHO-5 Well-Being Index) following overall, asymptomatic and symptomatic nocturnal hypoglycaemia at thresholds IG3.9 and IG3.0 in participants with type 1 diabetes

Separate analysis of nights with asymptomatic or symptomatic hypoglycaemia showed that the association was present after nights with asymptomatic hypoglycaemia (Table 2). In contrast, there was no difference in QoL following nights with symptomatic hypoglycaemia compared with nights without. The duration of asymptomatic nocturnal hypoglycaemia was positively associated with scores on EQ-5D VAS; participants at threshold IG3.9 scored 0.16 (95% CI 0.02, 0.31) higher (p = 0.030) on the EQ-5D VAS per 15 min increment in duration of preceding nocturnal hypoglycaemia. The same result was found at threshold IG3.0, with a score of 0.27 (95% CI 0.04, 0.51) higher (p = 0.022) per 15 min increment in duration of preceding nocturnal hypoglycaemia. The duration of symptomatic nocturnal hypoglycaemia was not associated with QoL (p = 0.84 for IG3.9; p = 0.80 for IG3.0). Neither the duration of asymptomatic nor symptomatic nocturnal events with glucose measurements solely between 3.0 mmol/l and 3.9 mmol/l was associated with scores on the ED-5D VAS (p = 0.70 and p = 0.05, respectively).

WHO-5 Well-Being Index

Neither previous nocturnal hypoglycaemia (IG3.9, p = 0.49; IG3.0, p = 0.17) (Table 2) nor the total duration of hypoglycaemia (IG3.9, p = 0.55; IG3.0, p = 0.53) were associated with QoL assessed by the WHO-5 Well-Being Index the subsequent day. Scores on the individual questions on the WHO Well-Being Index were not associated with prior nocturnal hypoglycaemia, including the question regarding quality of sleep (all p > 0.05).

Mood following nocturnal hypoglycaemia

All three mood states of the UWIST Mood Adjective Checklist scored similarly on days after hypoglycaemic nights and nights without hypoglycaemia (all p > 0.05). Likewise, the duration of hypoglycaemic nocturnal episodes was not associated with the mood the subsequent day. The results were similar at both threshold levels of hypoglycaemia (IG3.9 and IG3.0).

Effectiveness at work following nocturnal hypoglycaemia

Participants’ self-estimated effectiveness at work the day following a night with a hypoglycaemic event did not differ from the effectiveness on days after nights without hypoglycaemia (IG3.9, p = 0.75; IG3.0, p = 0.12). Furthermore, the duration of hypoglycaemic episodes (IG3.9, p = 0.43; IG3.0, p = 0.07) did not correlate to the participants’ self-estimated effectiveness.

State of awareness and QoL following nocturnal hypoglycaemia

Overall QoL assessed by the EQ-5D VAS (i.e. independent of occurrence of preceding nocturnal hypoglycaemia) decreased with decreasing state of hypoglycaemia awareness (EQ-5D VAS scores [mean ± SD]: normal awareness, 82.3 ± 15.3; impaired awareness, 80.9 ± 16.6; unawareness, 76.9 ± 16.7; p = 0.001). Participants with normal awareness assessed by the Hillerød method [18] had similar scores on the EQ-5D VAS following asymptomatic hypoglycaemic nights and non-hypoglycaemic nights (Table 3, Fig. 1). Participants with impaired awareness or unawareness (as characterised by the Hillerød method) scored 3.0 (95% CI 0.5, 5.5) higher on the EQ-5D VAS at IG3.9 (p = 0.017) for nights with vs without hypoglycaemia; the difference increased to 4.8 (95% CI 1.7, 7.9; p = 0.003) at IG3.0. Limiting the analysis to participants with hypoglycaemia unawareness according to the Hillerød method showed 10.4 (IG3.0 [95% CI 2.6, 18.1]) higher scores (p = 0.014) on the EQ-5D VAS the day following a hypoglycaemic asymptomatic night as compared with nights without hypoglycaemia (Table 3, Fig. 1). The same was seen in participants with reduced awareness according to the Clarke method, where EQ-5D VAS score was 7.4 (IG3.0 [95% CI 0.6, 14.1]) higher (p = 0.032) after asymptomatic hypoglycaemic nights compared with after non-hypoglycaemic nights. No effect was observed in participants classified as having impaired awareness by the Gold method (p = 0.34) at threshold IG3.0 (data not shown). There was no effect of symptomatic nocturnal hypoglycaemia in any of the awareness classes (Table 3). Moreover, there was no association between asymptomatic nocturnal events with glucose measurements solely between 3.0 mmol/l and 3.9 mmol/l and scores on the ED-5D VAS in any of the awareness classes (normal awareness, p = 0.29; impaired awareness and unawareness combined, p = 0.20).

Table 3 State of hypoglycaemia awareness (according to the Hillerød Method) and QoL (assessed by the EQ-5D VAS) following overall, asymptomatic and symptomatic episodes of nocturnal hypoglycaemia at the IG3.9 and IG3.0 thresholds
Fig. 1
figure 1

QoL assessed by the EQ-5D VAS following nocturnal hypoglycaemia at thresholds IG3.9 (ac) and IG3.0 (df) in 150 people with type 1 diabetes according to state of hypoglycaemia awareness (normal awareness, n = 69; impaired awareness, n = 59; unawareness, n = 22; assessed by the Hillerød method [18]). NH, hypoglycaemic nights; non-NH, non-hypoglycaemic nights. (a) Any episode of nocturnal hypoglycaemia (IG3.9). Comparison between groups (non-NH vs NH): normal awareness, p = 0.53; impaired awareness, p = 0.05; unawareness, p = 0.16. (b) Asymptomatic nocturnal hypoglycaemia (IG3.9). Non-NH vs NH: normal awareness, p = 0.44; impaired awareness, p = 0.07; unawareness, p = 0.15. (c) Symptomatic nocturnal hypoglycaemia (IG3.9). Non-NH vs NH: normal awareness, p = 0.94; impaired awareness, p = 0.71; unawareness, p = 0.92. (d) Any episode of nocturnal hypoglycaemia (IG3.0). Non-NH vs NH: normal awareness, p = 0.92; impaired awareness, p = 0.10; unawareness, p = 0.008. (e) Asymptomatic nocturnal hypoglycaemia (IG3.0). Non-NH vs NH: normal awareness, p = 0.70; impaired awareness, p = 0.31; unawareness, p = 0.014. (f) Symptomatic nocturnal hypoglycaemia (IG3.0). Non-NH vs NH: normal awareness, p = 0.58; impaired awareness, p = 0.15. There were too few symptomatic hypoglycaemic nights at IG3.0 in the group with hypoglycaemia unawareness for analysis. Data are mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, NH vs non-NH

Influence of daytime CGM-recorded glycaemia, nocturnal hyperglycaemia and SMBG on QoL

According to the CGM data, occurrence of both asymptomatic and symptomatic episodes of daytime hyperglycaemia (p = 0.80) and daytime hypoglycaemia (IG3.9, p = 0.30; IG3.0, p = 0.38) did not influence QoL reported according to the EQ-5D VAS scale the same evening. Adjustment for these daytime glucose excursions did not alter the effect of prior nocturnal asymptomatic hypoglycaemia on QoL. Furthermore, daytime TIR was not associated with changes in EQ-5D VAS (p = 0.12). Occurrence of daytime hypoglycaemia and hyperglycaemia was similar on days with and without prior nocturnal hypoglycaemia (p = 0.59 and p = 0.69, respectively). Moreover, TIR during the daytime was not related to occurrence of prior nocturnal hypoglycaemia (p = 0.66).

According to the EQ-5D VAS score the following day, QoL was not affected by either occurrence of nocturnal hyperglycaemia (p = 0.53) or occurrence of episodes with a glucose level between 4 mmol/l and 5 mmol/l (p = 0.27).

Lower fasting morning SMBG was associated with better QoL (EQ-5D VAS score [95% CI]: SMBG≤3.9, 83.1 [79.6, 86.5]; SMBG4–10, 82.1 [79.8, 84.5]; SMBG>10, 80.1 [77.7, 82.6]; p = 0.029). Separate analysis of the effect of fasting morning SMBG by awareness state showed that the effect of morning SMBG on QoL was only present for participants with normal awareness (EQ-5D VAS score [95% CI]: SMBG≤3.9, 86.2 [80.8, 91.6]; SMBG4–10, 83.9 [CI 80.5, 87.2]; SMBG>10, 81.4 [78.0, 84.8]; p = 0.042) and not for participants with impaired awareness or unawareness, as assessed by the Hillerød method (score [95% CI]: SMBG≤3.9, 80.8 [76.1, 85.5]; SMBG4–10, 80.6 [77.2, 84.0]; SMBG>10, 78.9 [75.3, 82.4]; p = 0.31).

Discussion

In the present study, QoL measured daily in the evening by the EQ-5D VAS was increased on days preceded by asymptomatic, but not symptomatic, nocturnal hypoglycaemia detected by blinded CGM. The QoL increment was ‘dose-dependent’ as it increased both with lower glucose nadir and longer duration of the hypoglycaemic episode, and it was transient as it was not present on the second day after the event. Furthermore, the effect was consistently confined to individuals with impaired hypoglycaemia awareness.

Overall, no clear glucose threshold for the effect of asymptomatic hypoglycaemia on QoL the following day was demonstrated. However, there was no effect of tight nocturnal glycaemic control and no significant effect of level 1 episodes with nadir between 3.0 and 3.9 mmol/l. This supports the present consensus on a glycaemic level of less than 3.0 mmol/l (level 2) for clinically important CGM-detected hypoglycaemia [23, 25].

To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the impact of spontaneous mild nocturnal hypoglycaemia detected by blinded CGM on day-to-day changes in QoL in type 1 diabetes. The finding that asymptomatic hypoglycaemia has an impact on QoL in real life may have great importance for people with type 1 diabetes as most episodes of nocturnal hypoglycaemic are asymptomatic [11, 12]. Since blinded CGM is used in our study, the effect of asymptomatic hypoglycaemia on QoL cannot be affected by participants having recognised the episodes. This suggests a biological effect of asymptomatic hypoglycaemia on QoL, which may be very different from the negative psychological effects previously reported to be associated with symptomatic nocturnal hypoglycaemia [3, 5, 6].

Other studies based on questionnaires retrospectively examining self-reported symptomatic hypoglycaemic episodes [3, 5, 6] or experimental studies exploring the effect of insulin-induced hypoglycaemia [7, 10, 27, 28] show that hypoglycaemia is associated with poorer QoL, mood and cognitive function. In our study, there was no impact of symptomatic nocturnal hypoglycaemia on QoL or other self-reported outcomes. This may be due to differences in the used methodologies, including QoL measures and timing of the assessments in relation to the hypoglycaemic events. Thus, if the assessment had instead been performed in the morning as in other studies [7, 10, 27, 28] it may have confirmed a negative impact of symptomatic hypoglycaemia.

People with impaired awareness had the lowest overall QoL assessed by the EQ-5D VAS. As these people have many-fold higher rates of severe hypoglycaemia, this is in accordance with the reported chronic negative impact of severe hypoglycaemia on QoL [5]. The positive impact of asymptomatic nocturnal hypoglycaemia on QoL was confined to the participants with the most impaired awareness who scored 10.3 higher on the EQ-5D VAS following nights with asymptomatic hypoglycaemia at IG3.0, a difference well above 7, which is regarded as clinically important [21].

The concept that people with impaired awareness may react differently to hypoglycaemia compared with those with normal awareness is supported by a number of studies [29, 30] investigating the effect of experimental insulin-induced mild hypoglycaemia [29] and state of awareness [30] on brain function. The studies show that adaptive changes (or habituation) in the cerebral responses to hypoglycaemia (with attenuated symptomatic response) may occur in individuals with impaired awareness [13, 14, 31,32,33,34]. These changes may include altered fuel transport and brain metabolism with use of alternative fuels during hypoglycaemia with neuroglycopenia. Lactate is suggested as an alternative fuel when glucose levels are low in the brain, albeit only in people with impaired awareness consistent with a metabolic adaptation to recurrent hypoglycaemia in the brain [35, 36]. Further changes include deactivation of areas associated with fear, distress and perception of hypoglycaemia as an important experience (e.g. amygdala, ventral striatum, medial orbital cortex) and increased activation of areas associated with reward sensations (lateral orbitofrontal cortex) [13, 33]. Thus, hypoglycaemia (with attenuated symptomatic response) in unaware individuals seemingly does not lead to appropriate fear and discomfort as seen in aware individuals but paradoxically may have a positive rewarding effect. Clinically, this is supported by studies demonstrating that individuals with unawareness of hypoglycaemia, who often are heavily exposed to asymptomatic hypoglycaemia, may be surprisingly unconcerned about their state of unawareness [37], and a subset of individuals with type 1 diabetes has low fear of hypoglycaemia despite carrying a high risk of severe episodes of hypoglycaemia [38]. Furthermore, interventions to reduce hypoglycaemia risk can be difficult to implement due to resistance in some individuals with impaired awareness [39]. Taken together, the evidence suggests a paradoxical pleasant or rewarding effect of hypoglycaemia on the brain in people with severely impaired awareness at an unconscious level.

The finding was not explained by an effect of prior nocturnal hypoglycaemia on daytime glycaemic profiles. Neither were they explained by tight nocturnal glycaemic control, subsequent glycaemic excursions during the day or the morning fasting plasma glucose, nor by the occurrence of preceding nocturnal hyperglycaemia. However, participants with normal awareness reported better QoL with lower fasting glucose values.

We only found a positive effect of previous nocturnal hypoglycaemia on QoL assessed by the ED-5D VAS scale and not by the WHO-5 Well-Being Index. This may be explained by the fact that the ED-5D VAS scale is a questionnaire designed to assess short-term changes in QoL [20] whereas the WHO-5 Well-Being Index is developed to show changes in QoL during a 1 week time span [19]. Therefore, the ED-5D VAS is probably more sensitive for detecting day-to-day variations resulting from previous nocturnal hypoglycaemia.

We were unable to show an effect of prior nocturnal hypoglycaemia (neither symptomatic nor asymptomatic) on mood states the following day in real life. An explanation may be that any changes in mood have recovered during the day before testing in the evening. This is supported by studies with insulin-induced mild symptomatic hypoglycaemia [10] and severe hypoglycaemia [8] in type 1 diabetes showing that recovery of mood states occurred within 30 min after restoration of euglycaemia following mild experimental hypoglycaemia [10] and within 1.5 days after an episode of severe hypoglycaemia [8].

Likewise, self-reported work efficiency was not affected by prior nocturnal hypoglycaemia. Cognitive performance is impaired during experimentally induced (both asymptomatic and symptomatic) hypoglycaemia in type 1 diabetes [28, 29] and is supposed to recover 20–75 min after restoration of euglycaemia [9, 40, 41], thus being less likely to be reflected in the evening assessment of work efficiency.

The strength of the present study is that it is based on blinded CGM data rather than SMBG. Furthermore, it is based on prospective assessment of the impact of spontaneous hypoglycaemia on QoL during daily life rather than the impact of retrospectively self-reported episodes of hypoglycaemia. A limitation to the study is the lack of data regarding sleep duration and quality. Another limitation shared with other CGM-based studies is the validity of nocturnal hypoglycaemic episodes recorded by CGM. Thus, in the early era of CGM the use of first-generation devices occasionally resulted in detection of spurious events. However, this problem is reduced with newer CGM technology including the device used in this study [42]. Moreover, the clear dose dependency of the effect of asymptomatic hypoglycaemia supports the validity of the result.

In conclusion, people with type 1 diabetes and impaired hypoglycaemia awareness transiently report improved QoL following nights with asymptomatic (but not symptomatic) hypoglycaemia. This effect, which is probably biological, increases with depth and duration of the hypoglycaemic event and lasts for only 1 day. The finding illustrates that there may exist a complex interplay between biological and psychological effects of hypoglycaemia, which may contribute to differences in perceptions of and attitudes to hypoglycaemia among people with diabetes. Understanding this day-to-day interplay is important when designing and implementing psychobehavioural interventions to prevent hypoglycaemia [43] and deserves further scientific attention.