Background

Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs) are a cluster of potentially traumatic events that occur in childhood and adolescence [1, 2]. ACEs can be defined as “those experiences which require significant adaptation by the develo** child in terms of psychological, social and neurodevelopmental systems, and which are outside of the normal expected environment” [3]. They include exposure to several forms of abuse and neglect, such as physical violence or emotional neglect, and some forms of household dysfunction, such as substance misuse and divorce, among others [1, 4]. ACEs are associated with risky behaviors, transmissible and non-transmissible diseases, injuries, and mental health issues, through direct and indirect pathways [5]. Regarding mental health illness, ACEs are associated with depressive symptoms, drug use, and conduct disorders, among others [6, 7].

ACEs affect a high proportion of the population. According to a retrospective study in twenty-three states of the United States, 61.55% of adults reported at least 1 ACE and 24.64% reported 3 or more ACEs [8]. In Latin America and the Caribbean, a study of violence against children found a prevalence of 30–60% for physical and emotional violence by caregivers, 17–61% for physical violence by students and 60 to 92% for emotional violence [9]. Similarly, in Colombia, an upper middle-income country from Latin America, it was found that adults have a prevalence of 64% for at least one ACE and 23% for four or more ACEs [10], and 29.3% of adolescents have suffered at least one traumatic event [11]. As for mental health outcomes, the most frequent illness related to ACEs is depression, with a risk increase of 2.7 times in women and 2.5 in men in comparison to those who have not been exposed [12]. Other associated mental health outcomes are use of psychotropic medication, tobacco dependence, and post-traumatic stress disorder [6, 13].

The developmental pathway from early adversity to depression has not been understood thoroughly yet, and it has not been either for many other outcomes. To account for the difference in long-term outcomes of people exposed to high levels of adversity, it is especially important to understand how people with exposure to early adversity differ in terms of protective factors. There are many ways trauma can influence the development of mental disorders, and its association with prosocial behavior has not been looked at in depth. ACEs may delay the development of prosocial behavior, increasing psychosocial adjustment problems [14]. For example, young adults who experienced childhood or adolescent maltreatment are more likely to exhibit psychopathic traits and less altruistic attitudes [15], and physical abuse is a positive predictor of deviant behaviors [16]. Besides, some evidence suggests that childhood adversity can lead to prosocial behaviors in adulthood through motivations like enhanced empathy, attempts to ameliorate the consequences of ACEs, finding a sense of purpose, and hel** others with similar experiences [17].

Prosocial behavior is the action of empathy and responsibility for the well-being and needs of others, it can be expressed as caring, volunteering or hel** others [18, 19]. The evidence reveals that acting in a prosocial and altruistic manner could increase someone’s self-concept [18], and a good self-concept in early adolescence will lead to better decisions during the transition to adulthood [20]. Further, elevated levels of prosocial behavior have been associated with better peer acceptance and academic achievement [21], have been reciprocally and directly associated with a positive affect [22], and negatively associated with both aggression and delinquency, even in early and late adolescence [23]. Moreover, a negative association between prosocial behavior and depressive symptoms has been reported in adolescents [24], however, adolescents with major depressive disorder seem to have increased prosocial behavior, which might be influenced by gender, relief from negative emotions or social-evaluative concern [24].

The evidence described above shows why it is needed to better understand the direct and indirect pathways leading to a major mental health outcome, such as depression. Both ACEs and prosocial behavior are associated with negative and positive outcomes regarding mental health and well-being in adulthood and adolescence. Nonetheless, the interaction between these variables and their role as protective or risk factors for the mental health of adolescent population is not well understood. Particularly, depression is associated with ACEs but there are mixed results regarding prosocial behavior, thus, understanding these paths could lead to the development of prevention strategies. Therefore, in the present study, we aimed to identify the relationship between ACEs, prosocial behavior and depression in adolescents from urban schools in low-resourced neighborhoods from Bogota, Colombia. We hypothesized that (1) ACEs had a positive association with depressive symptoms, (2) prosocial behavior had a negative association with both ACEs and depressive symptoms, and (3) prosocial behavior could modify the association between ACEs and depression after controlling for other variables.

Methods

A cross-sectional study was carried out through a virtual, anonymous, self-administered survey with secondary school students from seven charter schools located in low-resourced neighborhoods in Bogota (Colombia). The locations of the target population are characterized by being hotspots of social issues including crime, significant levels of domestic violence and poverty. The net coverage rate in secondary education is 71% and 41.4% in high school, and there are families who have been victims of internal displacement [25]. For over 15 years, our research team had worked on several projects with the alliance of charter schools involved in this survey, and this specific project had a higher degree of community participation. The schools were involved in all stages of research, from the design of the questionnaire to the consent process and publication (CGR and OLVR were school staff at the time).

The project was reviewed and approved by the Research Ethics Committee at the School of Medicine, Universidad de los Andes, Colombia (N20190726). Legal guardians were given information on the project at school meetings, and those willing to participate gave their written informed consent. The adolescents gave their assent to participate after their legal guardians had given their consent. Students were informed that their participation was voluntary, that answering or not answering had no academic repercussions, and that they could skip questions or finish the questionnaire when they wished to. As the questionnaire would be completely anonymous, the students were given written and verbal information on how to get help from the schools and the research team, in case they felt distressed. Besides, in Colombia, it is mandatory for the principal investigators to leave their personal information contact (telephone and e-mail) on the consent form for the legal guardians.

At the time of recruitment (second term, 2019), there were a total of 3710 secondary school students who were eligible to participate, and we estimated that at least 600 students were needed to detect differences of 5% points in the prevalence of the outcomes of different groups with a level of statistical significance of 5% in two-tailed hypothesis tests.

There were no exclusion criteria and all secondary school students were invited to participate. A total of 2918 adolescents participated (response rate of 77.45%), and 1576 of these were female (54.12%). 2724 students reported their age, with a mean of 13.81 years (Standard Deviation [SD] 1.82, Interquartile Range [IQR] 12–15). 2910 participants reported their school grade, 607 (20.86%) were in 6th grade, 598 (20.55%) in 7th grade, 483 (16.6%) in 8th grade, 410 (14.09%) in 9th grade, 482 (16.56%) in 10th grade, and 330 (11.34%) in 11th grade.

The questionnaire included screening instruments freely available and validated in Spanish, and many have been used in Colombia by other authors and by our team in some of the participant schools in previous research projects. They measure well-being, mental health, risk behaviors and symptoms of psychopathology, and were selected because the schools wanted to have diverse array of variables to monitor in their institutions afterwards, and our team wanted the questionnaires to be easily comparable with the latest National Mental Health Survey, as well as to be able to control for confounding variables when analyzing the data [26]. The questions that do not belong to a specific instrument and the selected instruments were reviewed by the researchers and members of the schools to ensure their acceptability for the participants, and the survey was administered in the computer rooms of the educational institutions. To ensure data validity and confidentiality, the survey process was supervised by teachers and counselors who had participated in the research project design, had worked with us to anticipate issues in the questionnaire, and allocated time for the computer rooms to be under their control with not too many students. Demographic variables included were age, sex, school, and education grade at enrollment (sixth to eleventh grades in our education system).

After reviewing instruments available elsewhere and in Colombia [27, 28], the team decided to conduct the assessment of ACEs by a series of customized yes/no questions about 19 different experiences (see Box 1 in Additional file 1), so a total score could be summed up from 0 to 19 points, and we considered 4 or more as the high-risk category. Prosocial behavior was evaluated with the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)—Prosocial subscale [29]. The prosocial score was calculated using five items (e.g., I am helpful if someone is hurt, upset or feeling ill), 0–2 points per item (not true, somewhat true or certainly true), and ranked as follows: 7–10, close to the average; 6, slightly lowered; 5, low and 0–4, very low [30]. We used the cut-offs available from the authors because there are not published cut-offs in Colombia, and we have found that the authors’ guidelines have clinical value in the projects we have used this instrument before.

Other instruments and variables assessed in this study were:

  • World Health Organization-Five Well-Being Index (WHO-5): A five-item questionnaire (e.g., over the last two weeks, I have felt cheerful and in good spirits?) using a 6-point Likert scale (from ‘all of the time’ to ‘at no time’), 0–5 points per item, with total scores ranging from 0 to 25. A total score of < 13 or a score ≤ 1 in any item indicates poor well-being [27]. We used the Spanish translation of the questionnaire that is freely available from the World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe [31], versions with a 4-point Likert scale have been studied in Colombian samples with acceptable overall psychometric performance [32], and the adult version of the scale has been suitable for children and adolescents [33].

  • Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS): A five-item questionnaire (e.g. In most ways my life is close to my ideal), using a 7-point Likert scale (from ‘Strongly agree’ to‘Strongly disagree’), 1–7 points per item, with total scores ranging from 5 to 35. Ranked as follows: 31–35, very satisfied; 26–30, satisfied; 21–25, a little satisfied, 20, neutral, 15–19, a little dissatisfied, 10–14, dissatisfied, and 5–9, very dissatisfied [34, 35].

  • Family Adaptability, Partnership, Growth, Affection, and Resolve (Family APGAR): A five-item questionnaire (e.g., I find that my family accepts my wishes to take on new activities or make changes in my life-style) using a 5-point Likert scale ( from ‘almost always’ to ‘hardly ever’) (0–4 points per item), with total scores ranging from 0 to 20. Ranked as follows: 0–9 indicates severe dysfunction; 10–13, moderate dysfunction; 14–17, mild dysfunction, and ≥ 18 is considered good functioning [36, 37].

  • Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)—Difficulties scores [29, 30]: The 20-item self-report SDQ items were divided between 4 subscales of 5 items each (0–2 points per item): Hyperactivity (e.g., I am restless, I cannot stay still for long), Emotional Symptoms (e.g., I have many fears, I am easily scared), Conduct Problems (e.g., I take things that are not mine from home, school or elsewhere), and Peer Problems (e.g., Other children or young people pick on me or bully me). Each item score from 0 to 2 (not true, somewhat true, or certainly true), each subscale has a score ranging from 0 to 10, and all scores are summed up to generate a total difficulties score from 0 to 40. The scores for total difficulties were ranked as: close to the average (0–14 points), 15 to 17 slightly raised (15–17 points), high (18–19 points), and very high (20–40 points). We used the cut-offs available from the authors as was already stated for the SDQ – Prosocial subscale.

  • Self-reporting questionnaire, 20-item version (SRQ-20) [38]: A 20-item questionnaire using a yes/no format, with possible total scores ranging from 0 to 20 (positive screen ≥ 8), as well as an anxiety subscale score (0–10 points, positive screen ≥ 5) and a depression subscale score (0–13 points, positive screen ≥ 7). Some example items are: ‘Do you sleep badly?’, ‘Are you easily tired?’, ‘Do you feel unhappy?’. This is a widely used screening instrument for anxiety/depressive disorders, and the present study adopted the cut-off points used in the Colombian National Mental Health Survey (NMHS) 2015 [26, 39].

  • Self-injurious behavior: lifetime prevalence of non-suicidal and suicidal types, and number of episodes per type (once, 2–5 times, 6 or more times).

A STATA® database was generated, and the quality and veracity of the information was checked. Most variables had complete data for more than 95% of the sample, and therefore missing values were excluded from each analysis. Central tendency and dispersion measures, as well as graphical inspection of the data, were used to detect possible extreme values and unexpected distributions. Internal consistency of the scales was evaluated using Cronbach’s alpha.

Bivariate association was tested using the Spearman correlation coefficient, Z tests and Chi-square tests, according to the characteristics of each variable. The main bivariate association shown in this manuscript is the Spearman correlation coefficient between ACEs and prosocial behavior. These bivariate analyses were used to find what variables could be potential confounders as they are simultaneously associated with depression and either ACEs or prosocial behavior. To reduce bias, all multivariate association analyses were adjusted for potential confounding variables through logistic regression, and the possibility of multicollinearity was ruled out using Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) analyses and conducting additional regression models excluding variables that could duplicate questions. We used depression as the outcome, ACEs and prosocial behavior as independent variables, and include demographics and the rest of the mental health variables as covariates to ascertain whether the association between the outcome and independent variables was significant in the presence of other associations. Crude and adjusted Odds Ratios (OR) were obtained in these regression models, and the interaction term for ACEs and prosocial behavior was tested separately as well.

Results

Table S1 (Additional file 1) displays the distribution of all mental health indicators for the whole sample, with internal consistency for total scores and subscales. All scales and subscales used had a good internal consistency, except some of the SDQ subscales. Only 361 respondents reported no exposure to ACEs, 2557 (87.63%) reported one or more, and 1087 (37.25%) reported four or more ACEs. On average, participants reported being exposed to 3.15 ACEs (SD: 2.55, median: 3, IQR: 1–5). For all ACEs, missing data was less than 5%, except for the question on partner violence as 1038 (35.57%) have not had a partner ever. As shown in Table S2 (Additional file 1), the most prevalent ACE was witnessing violent acts in their neighborhood or near the school, followed by parents separated or divorced and lack of support, love, or protection. As for the prosocial behavior, 2872 students had a mean score of 7.12 (SD: 1.75, median: 7.0, IQR: 6–8). Out of those, 1848 (64.35%) were classified as close to the average; 503 (17.51%) as slightly lowered; 342 (11.91%) as low; and 179 (6.23%) as very low. Finally, the SRQ-20 showed a high proportion of the sample had seven or more depression symptoms (19.00%).

Tables 1 and 2 show association tests between our main variables (ACEs in Table 1, and prosocial behavior in Table 2) and the other mental health indicators. Exposure to ACEs was associated to all tested variables, with higher levels of exposure to adversity among females. The adolescents exposed to four or more ACEs obtained lower scores of well-being and satisfaction with life, higher levels of internalizing and externalizing symptoms, and higher prevalence of self-injurious behaviors. Similarly, prosocial behavior was associated with most variables, except the SDQ subscale on emotional problems and the prevalence of suicide attempts, and lower levels of prosocial behavior were found among men. Those with low prosocial behavior (score 0–5) had significantly lower scores of well-being, satisfaction with life and higher prevalence of self-injury.

Table 1 Association between adverse childhood experiences, demographics, and mental health indicators
Table 2 Association between prosocial behavior, demographics, and mental health indicators

The Spearman correlation coefficient between ACEs and prosocial behavior was − 0.02 (p-value = 1.000), so they were not associated in this sample. The logistic regression models (Table 3) found a significant association of a positive depression screening with ACEs (model 1, OR 2.21; 95% CI 1.67–2.94), but not with prosocial behavior (model 2, OR 0.97, 95% CI 0.73–1.28). Besides that, the association between ACEs and depressive symptoms was not modified when including prosocial behavior in the model (model 3), and the test for their interaction did not show statistically significant results (data not shown in Table, OR 0.93, 95% CI 0.54–1.63).

Table 3 Logistic regression models showing the association of selected variables with a positive depression screening

These models adjusted the associations for several confounding variables that showed association with ACEs and prosocial behaviors (previously described), and with depression scores (data not shown). Although all variables shown in models have a statistically significant association, some of them have a larger effect size; well-being and general psychopathology have the strongest associations with a positive depression screening. As there is correlation between the variables included in the model, we tested for multicollinearity, and found every variable has a low VIF (data not shown, all VIF values were between 1.36 and 1.93).

Discussion

This study aimed to identify the relationship between ACEs, prosocial behavior, and depression in an adolescent population from urban schools in low-resourced neighborhoods. First, we identified that more than 35% of the adolescents had been exposed to four or more ACEs, and almost one out of every five teenagers were classified as having a low or very low results in prosocial behavior. These were alarming results; they mean a huge proportion of adolescents living under similar conditions are at high risk of psychopathology and other serious outcomes. Even so, they do not mean adolescents under vulnerable conditions are unique to the Colombian population. Similarly, in a Brazilian study, 33.3% of the 15–19-year-old high school students reported four or more ACEs [40], and 72% of adolescents aged 10–16 years in Malawi reported the same [41]. As for the levels of prosocial behavior found in our sample, they are also comparable to those found in similar studies. For example, in a study from Honduras, the authors found a mean score of 7.93 (SD 1.91) in the prosocial behavior subscale of parent-version SDQ [42], and authors in China reported a mean score of 7.24 (SD 2.19) using the same instrument [46]. Contrary to our findings, Eli et al. found a statistically significant association between high levels of depression and low levels of both prosocial behavior and resilience [47]. However, this study did not measure/control confounding variables beyond some demographics and the three main variables. There are other studies that identified a statistically significant association between child maltreatment and prosocial behavior, and empathy and gratitude as significant mediators in that association [48]. Nevertheless, that study did not measure psychopathology or any other variables besides demographics and the aforementioned.

Possible explanations for the findings in this study are: The lack of association between exposure to adversity and prosocial behavior; a problem with the measurement of those variables in this sample; and the presence of other unidentified mediators that could serve as protective factors from adversity. We were able to identify at least two other studies that measured both ACEs, prosocial behavior and psychopathology. Bevilacqua et al. evaluated the association between ACEs, psychopathology and prosocial behavior [14], using the parent-version of the SDQ at ages 3, 5, 7, 11 and 14. The authors found that a higher number of ACEs predicted worse mental health and prosocial outcomes, which were evident by age 3 and persisted until adolescence [14]. There is another study that evaluated aggressive behavior and prosocial skills in children exposed to intimate partner violence through preschool and early-school years; their results indicated that there was a cross-domain relation between aggressive behavior problems and prosocial skills, exacerbated by early intimate partner violence exposure [49]. Preschool-age aggressive behavior negatively influenced prosocial skills development during the early school years. Moreover, preschool-age intimate partner violence exposure was linked to decreased early school-age prosocial skills and increased aggressive behavior problems [49]. We do not think there were measurement biases in our study because our findings are quite like other authors’ reports in the prevalence of early adversity and the levels of prosocial behavior. It may be possible that differences are due to the use of different instruments for prosociality as some of them could grasp a more complete picture of the underlying construct.

Exposure to early adversity plays a key role in depression and other mental health outcomes among adolescent population, and its effects may not be alleviated by the presence or positive mental health constructs like prosocial behavior. New studies are struggling to understand the protective effect of benevolent childhood experiences, resilience, and many other positive psychology constructs or protective factors. Those studies need to be conducted in a way that facilitates a better understanding of what constructs are underlying traits, events, neurobiological states, and outcomes; for example, depending on the framework of a research project, prosociality could be modeled either as a trait to explain differences in depression, or as a variable with bidirectional association with depressive symptoms throughout time which in turn explains satisfaction with life as an outcome in the long term. When generating models for understanding the complex phenomenon of mental health, these findings support that they should consider multiple variables and their interactions, even more so in the developmental process of children and adolescents.

As with any cross-sectional study, our project has some limitations. The measurement of prosocial behavior was performed with a subscale of the SDQ (a screening instrument for general psychopathology) and we obtained a low internal consistency; there are other scales that are specifically designed to measure that construct, and they could be considered more thorough, such as the Prosociality Scale [50]. Its reduced version has been used in Colombian [51], Argentinian [52] and Japanese [53] adolescents; however, it is designed for young adults [54]. There is a widespread need for locally validated measures that allow comparison between populations using the same instruments. Besides, this study has weaknesses inherent to cross-sectional studies, such as the inability to measure incidence, difficulty in making causal inferences, associations that are difficult to interpret, and susceptibility to non-response from the participants.

Nonetheless, our response rate was remarkably high in comparison to similar studies on school mental health, and a high response rate allowed us to reduce errors in the estimation of the associations found. Recall bias might be another issue as we asked retrospectively about ACEs using questions adapted from standardized measures available in other studies [27]. This is an inevitable limitation, but longitudinal follow-up and assessment of ACEs would be costly in many ways as well. Our instrument captured 19 different ACEs and was reviewed by experts and members of the schools’ communities to ensure it would be appropriate for adolescents. We adapted questions from multiple questionnaires to assess as many ACEs as possible. There are many instruments available as adversity measures, but few of them have been tested in Colombian adolescents [27, 28].

Conclusions

ACEs were significantly associated with a positive depression screening when controlling for prosocial behavior and confounding variables such as well-being, satisfaction with life and family functioning. Thus, implementation of trauma-informed mental health promotion strategies at schools, primary care and community mental health centers, is cornerstone to improve the well-being and positive mental health outcomes during adolescence. Furthermore, acknowledgement of the associations -or their absence- between these variables is key when planning how to prevent mental health illnesses in this population and settle regular follow-ups for those at higher risk, as teens exposed to multiple ACEs.

Models generated within the ecological framework could help to better understand the interaction of factors at different levels as well as to help design and carry out effective programs for prevention of mental disorders. Trauma-informed school mental health should not be understood as the performance of universal screening, that approach might be impossible for some schools and difficult to handle for many more of them. As some variables are not modifiable, such as age, sex, and exposure to ACEs, interventions could be directed to improve adolescents’ well-being and health by enhancing factors like satisfaction with life, family functionality, and prosocial behavior. Such an approach might seem logical and useful, but it may not be the most efficient way to address the problem.

Participant charter schools in this study could prioritize their needs and focus their resources on an optimal way for different profiles of their students. Selective and indicated prevention strategies or interventions could help some of the children, but they would be costly for most schools in Colombia. Those strategies should be prioritized within the healthcare system, but there is a burden the healthcare system cannot handle and early diagnosis without treatment is only leading to a larger mental health gap, even more so under inequity conditions. Another strategy to overcome this gap is community participation for the development of interventions that could promote mental health in a universal manner, and positive psychology constructs serving as protective factors and mental health determinants should be the priority of such efforts in research and practice.

Finally, in this sample, prosocial behavior was not associated with ACEs nor depressive symptoms and did not significantly modify the association between ACEs and depression. Intervening prosociality in adolescents exposed to multiple ACEs might not be enough to prevent or treat depression, as they may need trauma-focused interventions that address other issues. On the contrary, intervening prosociality in adolescents with low exposure to ACEs might be useful to increase well-being and other positive mental health outcomes. It should be noted that the design and implementation of these interventions requires further research and a multidisciplinary approach, which includes participation of the education sector.