Introduction

Urbanization is associated with several benefits, but also challenges. “City life” is, amongst others, linked to improved access to education, culture, health institutions, and employment1. Simultaneously, urban living comes with an increased risk of stress-related mental disorders, such as depression or anxiety disorders2.

Interestingly, urban living is associated with increased amygdala activity in a stress paradigm, which is a key region for emotional processing and threat detection3,4. Several factors could mediate this association. Apart from social factors (e.g., social density, social isolation), environmental factors (e.g., decreased exposure to nature experiences and increased exposure to air/noise pollution) have been associated with negative impacts on physiological and psychological health5,6.

Greater exposure to green space (GS) has been shown to be associated with improved mood, perceived general health, and increased physical activity, whereas a negative association was reported for obesity and body mass index (BMI)7,8,9,10. Furthermore, GS has been reported to play a part in everyday co** with stress: more GS in residential areas has been associated with a steeper decrease in the cortisol level during the day and a lower level of self-reported stress11. Only one functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study has investigated the association between GS and emotional wellbeing. GS exposure (as measured with ecological momentary assessments) was especially beneficial in terms of mood improvement for urban dwellers who depicted lower, possibly less regulatory activity in the dorsal prefrontal cortex during processing of aversive emotional cues. That study suggested that urban green space (UGS) exposure might be a compensating factor for reduced neuronal regulation12. It is important to note, that population subgroups might benefit in different ways, caused by factors like e.g. age, level of education and social support13,14,15.

Whereas GS in cities seems to promote mental wellbeing, urban air pollution has been shown to be related to neurotoxicity, neurodegeneration, worse cognitive performance, an increased risk for the recurrence of depressive symptoms, and suicide16,17,18,19,20,21. Across different air pollutants, particulate matter (PM) has been identified to be among the most prevalent and harmful22. Though less often investigated than PM, ambient gaseous nitric oxides NOx and/or NO2 have been associated with an increased risk of dementia, cerebrovascular disease, neurodegenerative syndromes, and poorer cognitive development in children23,24,25.

A third environmental risk factor in urban settings is noise. For example, noise annoyance has been reported to lead to a chronic stress response associated with an increased release of stress hormones26. Several studies have indicated that noise might contribute to the development of metabolic and cardiovascular diseases, as well as to autonomic imbalance and vascular dysfunction27,28,29,30. Hence, GS, air pollution, and noise pollution are environmental determinants that distinguish urban areas from more rural areas, and have been shown to influence mental health: salutogenic for GS, and pathogenic for air pollution and noise pollution. A fMRI study on how air pollution and noise pollution may endanger psychological wellbeing has not been undertaken. Here, we assessed brain activation during a social-stress paradigm in male urban dwellers. Due to the menstrual cycle and hormonal fluctuations in women, stress responses have been shown to differ from men, which is why we included only men for this study31,32,33. We hypothesized that a higher percentage of residential GS and lower values of air pollution and noise pollution are associated with enhanced processing of neural stress.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Verbal-stress rating and cortisol concentration for t1 to t4 are depicted in Fig. 1a,b, respectively. The stress rating did not change across the four sampling time points (p = 0.455, η2 = 0.023), but post hoc t-tests demonstrated an increase from pre-stress to post-stress (t(41) =  − 9.346, p < 0.001, d = − 1.35) and a subsequent decrease to the next time point (t(41) = 7.454, p < 0.001, d = 0.989). The cortisol concentration changed over time (F (2.026, 74.967) = 3.158, p = 0.048, η2 = 0.079), but post hoc t-tests were not significant between any of the four sampling time points (p > 0.05). Heart rate changed across the three time points measured (F (1.129, 38.396) = 8.238, p = 0.005, η2 = 0.195), with a significant increase during stress (t(36) =  − 8.353, p < 0.001, d = − 1.116), followed by a decrease during the scan after the stress task (t(38) = 10.389, p < 0.001, d = 1.034) (Fig. 1c). Descriptive values are reported in Tables S1 and S2.

Figure 1
figure 1

Mean (SD) for (a) verbal-stress rating, (b) cortisol concentration (in nmol/L) and (c) heart rate. VR = verbal-stress rating; Cort = cortisol concentration; t = time point; HR = heart rate; r = resting state; *p < 0.001.

Descriptive statistics for the mean value of percentage GS, air pollution, and noise pollution are listed in the supplement (Tables S3 and S4). Cortisol AUCi showed a trend for an association with GS in a buffer of 5000 m (r =  − 0.308, p = 0.053). That is, the higher the amount of residential GS in a buffer of 5000 m, the lower was the cortisol AUCi in response to the social stressor. All other correlations were not significant (Table S5). When adjusted for age, the trend disappeared (r = − 0.184, p = 0.263). PM2.5 and PM10 showed negative associations with GS buffers ≥ 1500 m (− 0.74 < r <  − 0.47, all p ≤ 0.005, Bonferroni-corrected) (Table S6). For NO2 and NOx, negative correlations were found with GS buffers ≥ 1500 m (− 0.57 < r <  − 0.46, all p ≤ 0.005, Bonferroni-corrected).

Main effect of stress according to fMRI

Comparison of stress with control conditions revealed stronger activity in distributed brain regions (p < 0.05, TFCE-corrected) (Fig. 2). These included regions central to stress processing, such as the thalamus, frontoinsular cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala. Deactivation was found in the bilateral rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), ventral striatum (VS), ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), left frontal pole, and left occipital cortex (OC).

Figure 2
figure 2

Main effect of stress > control overlaid on the 1-mm Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template (values represent uncorrected t-values). Activated brain regions are shown in red → yellow, and deactivated brain regions in blue → light-blue. R, right; L, left.

Association between GS and brain activity

GS within a buffer of 5000 m (Fig. 3d) showed an association with stress-related activation in the right insular cortex, superior parietal cortex, and lateral occipital cortex (p < 0.017, FWE-corrected for comparison of nine buffer sizes). These data indicated that participants with a higher percentage of GS in a buffer of 5000 m around their residence tended to have stronger activation in these regions during the stress condition compared with that in the control condition. At a more lenient FWE-corrected threshold of p < 0.05, additional associations appeared in the right ventromedial (vmPFC) and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC), and ventral striatum (VS), left and right amygdala, precuneus, ventral posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), hippocampus, and ventral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), as well as in the left lateral occipital cortex, superior parietal cortex, fusiform gyrus and insular cortex. Using the more lenient FWE-corrected threshold, brain regions found for 4000 m (Fig. 3c) were comparable with those found for 5000 m, additionally showing associations in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and right fusiform cortex, whereas the buffers of 1500 m (Fig. 3a) and 2000 m (Fig. 3b) were associated positively with activity in the right vlPFC. GS within a buffer of 250 m, 500 m, 1000 m, 2500 m, and 3000 m, as well as the distance between the residence and the nearest GS ≥ 2 ha, were not associated with stress-related brain activation.

Figure 3
figure 3

Stronger activity related to green space in a buffer of (a) 1500 m, (b) 2000 m, (c) 4000 m, and (d) 5000 m for the contrast stress > control overlaid on the 1-mm MNI template. Red-to-yellow indicates p < 0.05 (FWE-corrected) and green indicates p < 0.017 (FWE-corrected for comparison of nine buffer sizes). R, right; L, left.

The mean time series of the significant buffers 1500 m, 2000 m, 4000 m, and 5000 m were not correlated with BMI (see Supplement Table S7 for descriptive statistics and Pearson correlation).

Association between air pollution and brain activity

In contrast to GS, the concentration of PM2.5 (Fig. 4a) and PM10 (Fig. 4b) in residential areas showed a negative association with stress-related brain activation (p < 0.047, FWE-corrected for comparison of two PM values). This association was more pronounced for PM2.5 than for PM10, and comprised several regions in both hemispheres: frontoinsular cortex, hippocampus, amygdala, VS, inferior parietal cortex, thalamus, precuneus, PCC, dACC, dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC), ventrolateral PFC (vlPFC), and vmPFC. That is, participants exposed to a higher PM in their residential area had weaker stress-related activation in those brain regions. An association for the concentration of NO2 and NOx in residential areas was not found.

Figure 4
figure 4

Weaker activity related to (a) PM2.5 and (b) PM10 for contrast stress > control (p < 0.047, corrected for multiple comparisons) overlaid on the 1-mm MNI template. R, right; L, left.

Association between noise and brain activity

An association was not found between residential noise during the day and night for a buffer of 50 m or for 100 m.

Discussion

We wished to examine the association between urban environmental variables and brain activity during a social-stress paradigm. Successful stress induction was judged by an increase in verbal stress ratings and heart rate, as well as by a significant main effect for cortisol concentration. Overall, the analysis of the main effect of stress showed a similar activation and deactivation pattern to that stated in studies on stress processing34. In terms of environmental variables, we showed stronger activation in brain regions involved in emotion-based regulation of stress compared with that in the control condition for participants with higher availability of GS, particularly within a buffer of 5000 m, whereas the buffers of 1500 m, 2000 m, and 4000 m showed stronger activation at a more lenient threshold. In contrast, less activity in a more extended set of brain regions was found for participants with a higher amount of PM (especially PM2∙5) at their place of residence, whereas there was no association with NO or noise pollution.

For many people, nature provides an opportunity to escape the stress of daily life and to regenerate their cognitive resources35,36. However, until now there has been no distinct operationalization of what constitutes “restorative” GS37. Ekkel and de Vries stated that cumulative opportunity metrics (e.g., percentage of GS, averaged Normalized Difference Vegetation Index) show a more consistent association with health than metrics of residential proximity accessibility (e.g., distance to nearest GS, GS availability in pre-set distance)38. Our results showed no association between stress-related brain activation and the amount of GS for the small, “walkable” buffers of 250 m, 500 m, or 1000 m. However, we found a positive association with larger buffers, particularly 1500 m, 2000 m, and 4000 m (not corrected for the number of buffers), and 5000 m (corrected for the number of buffers), which illustrated a stronger and more widespread activity pattern with increasing buffer size. The smaller buffers of 1500 m and 2000 m were associated with stress-related activation in the right vlPFC, an area well known for cognitive control of emotional processing39,40. The larger buffers of 4000 m and 5000 m were associated with a more diffuse activation pattern, including the insular cortex (which is implicated in emotional perception and salience detection of cues), the vmPFC, vlPFC, dlPFC, and vACC (which are important for emotion regulation), amygdala and ventral striatum (which are related to the perception of emotional features of a stimuli), the fusiform cortex (which is important for facial emotion recognition) as well as the precuneus and PCC (which are related to self-referential thought)34,39,41. In general, it seems that a larger amount of GS was associated with stronger activation in brain regions that are important for regulating emotions when processing a stressful task. Surprisingly, the GS buffers of 2500 m and 3000 m did not show an association with these brain regions. However, when applying a more lenient, uncorrected threshold, we observed a similar association with activation of the right vlPFC for these buffers as that found for 1500 m and 2000 m. Interestingly, for the buffer of 3000 m, additional regions appeared to be in agreement with the regions reported for 4000 m and 5000 m, which may indicate a shift in neural associations from more rostral to dorsal stress-related brain activity with an increasing percentage of GS.

The observed associations with activity in the right vlPFC (buffers: 1500 m, 2000 m), as well as in the insular cortex, vmPFC, vlPFC, dlPFC, vACC, precuneus, and PCC (buffers: 4000 m, 5000 m) may suggest a supportive effect of GS on co** with stressful events. However, we cannot infer how the amount of GS would exert such effects. Interaction with nature becomes less likely with increasing distance for people to reach GS42. This matters especially for effects found within the two largest buffers. As such, our results might partly reflect an indirect association between GS and processing of neural stress, which may be influenced by the role of GS in filtering air pollution43. This hypothesis is supported by two results. First, we found a negative association between the amount of GS and air pollution for buffers ≥ 1500 m. Second, weaker activation was found for increased air pollution in the bilateral frontoinsular cortex, vmPFC, vlPFC, dlPFC, amygdala, hippocampus, precuneus, and PCC, which mirrored the pattern of brain regions that showed a positive association with GS. However, deactivations associated with PM comprised additional (inferior parietal cortex, thalamus, dACC) and larger areas than activations associated with green space. Interestingly, most of these regions are central nodes of three well-studied functional-connectivity networks in relation to the acute stress response: the salience network (dACC, insula, amygdala), default mode network (PCC, precuneus, hippocampus, vmPFC, inferior parietal cortex), and central executive network (dlPFC)34. Though fMRI research on the influence of air pollution on stress processing is still missing, a previous study employed the Trier Social Stress Test outside the scanner and reported that higher PM2.5 concentrations in the residing neighborhood were associated with a greater autonomic response as indicated by lower heart rate variability and higher skin conductance levels44. This suggests that air pollution (at least PM2.5) potentially inhibits an appropriate stress response, which coincides with our results in which PM may lead to a general attenuation of stress-related activity in these networks.

An association could be localized for the smaller PM2.5 in considerably more brain regions than for the larger PM10. This observation could be explained by an easier passage of the blood–brain barrier by smaller particles compared to larger ones, but also by a different chemical composition of the two particle sizes22,45. A previous study reported that PM2.5 accounted for 65% of PM10, suggesting reducing especially PM2.5 is crucial for improving air quality46. For the environmental variables we used, PM2.5 was also present in PM10 and, thus, may have driven the effect of PM10.

Surprisingly, the activity changes of the amygdala, a region which is associated with emotional processing, were opposite to what we would have expected to see, based on the assumption that green space promotes adaptive stress processing (hence we would have expected less activation of amygdala) and air pollution weakens adaptive stress processing (hence we would have expected stronger activation of amygdala)39. Interesting to note is that while for PM2.5 the significant area covered mostly anterior-lateral amygdala regions, whereas for GS only anterior parts appeared significant. The amygdala is a complex of several nuclei with different in- and output regions, which have been shown to not only correspond to negative, but also positive emotional stimuli47,48. However, segregating the amygdala into anatomically and functionally specific regions has been challenging and could be an interesting research question for future studies to consider49.

Additionally, PM has been discussed to impair the central nervous system by stimulation of pro-inflammatory cytokines and oxidative stress, which lead to neuronal loss50,66. Noise data were based on a continuation of the “strategic noise maps” of Berlin created in 2017. These maps combine information on the mean noise pollution of the main sources of urban noise (e.g., road traffic, subway traffic, noise from industry and commerce, and air traffic) during the night (22:00–06:00) and day (06:00–22:00). Noise data were extracted within a buffer of 50 m and 100 m of the participant’s street address to take into account different levels of noise exposure depending on the orientation of the place of residence. For more information on the residential area studied, see Supplemental Material and Fig. S1 and S2.

fMRI task

The ScanSTRESS task was used to induce acute social stress by carrying out figure rotations and mathematical subtraction tasks while being observed by a two-person panel who provided verbal negative feedback (incorporating elements of social-evaluative threat and unpredictability)60. Time pressure was introduced by an algorithm that reduced the available time to solve the set tasks depending on performance during previous trials and by presenting the remaining time via a visualized countdown (equals uncontrollability). In the case of a mistake, the comment “Incorrect!” was presented on the screen. In the case of a correct answer, but slow performance, the comment “Work faster!” was presented on the screen. During the control condition, participants merely had to match figures and numbers.

The task started after instructions from the panel with a practice run that included a control condition for figure rotation and subtraction and a stress condition for figure rotation and subtraction. Each run took 30 s, with a 10-s break in-between. After the practice run, the panel provided negative verbal feedback to increase subjective stress. Afterwards, the experimental run started with a control block consisting of 60 s of figure rotation, a 20-s break, 60 s of subtraction, a 20-s break, followed by a stress block with 60 s of figure rotation, a 20-s break, and 60 s of subtraction. This order was repeated twice so that the total duration of the task, including practice and experimental runs, spanned ~ 15 min. The adapted ScanSTRESS paradigm was programmed and presented with Presentation 18.1 provided by Neurobehavioral Systems (http://www.neurobs.com/).

ScanSTRESS was embedded in a larger scanning protocol which comprised acquisition of structural scans (MPRAGE), a fieldmap, two resting state scans (pre- and post-stress) and a working-memory task with emotional distracter images (Fig. 5)67.

Figure 5
figure 5

The scanning procedure (a) comprised a T1 MPRAGE at the start, two resting-state (RS) scans (one before and one after the stress task), followed by another functional-task scan. Saliva samples (S) were taken at eight time points: t0 = upon arrival, t1 = pre-scan, t2 = post resting-state 1, t3 = post-stress, t4 = post resting-state 2. Three more saliva samples were taken: after a second task (t5), and before (t6) and after (t7) the debriefing. HR(V) = heart rate and heart rate variability. The ScanSTRESS task (b) consisted of two runs inside the MRI scanner: a practice run without scanning (duration: 2:50 min) and an experimental run during scanning (duration: 11:00 min).

Eight saliva samples were taken throughout the entire scanning protocol: two before scanning (t0, t1), one directly before ScanSTRESS (t2), three after ScanSTRESS in-between other scans (t3, t4, t5), and two after scanning (t6, t7). During these timepoints, participants also rated their subjective feeling of stress on a scale from 1 (“very low”) to 10 (“very high”). Heart rate was recorded during ScanSTRESS, as well as during resting-state scans preceding and following the stress task.

Descriptive and statistical analyses

The cortisol concentration in saliva, stress rating, and heart rate were tested with a repeated-measures ANCOVA, followed by post hoc one-tailed paired-sample t-tests, using SPSS 25.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA), including age and BMI as covariates. Effect sizes for t-tests were calculated using the website http://www.psychometrica.de/effect_size.html. The cortisol concentration as well as the subjective stress rating were analyzed for four time points (t1–t4). We excluded t0 (because it was outside the scanner environment) and t5 to t7 (because the cortisol concentration during these time points was influenced by administration of an emotional-distracter task)67. One participant was excluded from analyses for t1 and t4, and two participants for t2, due to missing values for the cortisol concentration. The cortisol area under the curve with respect to increase (AUCi) was calculated, which was used for Pearson correlations with environmental variables68. Two participants were excluded from the AUCi analysis due to missing values for the cortisol concentration at one or more of the sampling time points. Heart rate was not recorded for three participants during resting-state scan before the stress task (r1), for three participants during stress task and for one participant during resting-state scan after the stress task (r2), providing 37 values for the t-test including r1 and stress and 39 values for the t-test including stress and r2.

Acquisition and processing of fMRI data

During the ScanSTRESS task, gradient-echo planar images were acquired on a 3-T scanner (Trio; Siemens, Munich, Germany) with a 32-channel head coil using the following parameters: 426 volumes; repetition time (TR) = 1560 ms; echo time (TE) = 25 ms; flip angle = 65°. Twenty-eight slices of 3-mm isotropic voxels were acquired sequentially in descending order, and auto-aligned parallel to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure line. A high-resolution T1-weighted image (magnetization prepared-rapid gradient echo; 1-mm isotropic voxels; TR = 1900 ms; TE = 2.52 ms; flip angle = 9°) and a fieldmap image (3-mm isotropic voxels; TR = 434 ms; TE = 5.19 ms; flip angle = 60°) were acquired for registration. Preprocessing was carried out using FMRIB Software Library (FSL), Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs), and Independent Component-Analysis based Automatic Removal of Motion Artifacts (ICA-AROMA)69,71,71. Subject-level analyses were carried out in FSL using the general linear model, in which stress blocks were compared with control blocks. Group-level differences in activity between stress blocks and control blocks were assessed with a one-sample t-test, as well as the association of these differences with geographical data, each time including age as a covariate. Then, the resulting t-statistical maps underwent threshold-free cluster enhancement using the default parameter settings (H = 2, E = 0.5, C = 6), and significance testing was carried out with permutation testing (4000 iterations) using TFCE_mediation (https://github.com/trislett/TFCE_mediation)72. In the latter step, a null distribution of random results was generated against which empirical findings were tested. This strategy resulted in statistical images that were Family Wise Error (FEW)-corrected across the whole brain at p < 0.05 for the main effect of stress. Taking the mutual correlation between the nine buffer sizes of GS into account (average r = 0.50), the Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold was p = 0.017 (as calculated with SISA; http://www.quantitativeskills.com/sisa/). Taking the mutual correlation between the two PM and NO values into account (r = 0.92 for PM, r = 0.99 for NO), the Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold was p = 0.047 for PM and p = 0.05 for NO. Voxel-wise uncorrected (t) and corrected (TFCE p) statistical maps of our analyses are available on NeuroVault (http://neurovault.org/collections/9333).

In order to test possible associations of GS-related changes in brain activity with Body Mass Index (BMI), mean time series from the whole brain activity were extracted at a lenient threshold of p < 0.05 from significant buffers and correlated with BMI in a Pearson correlation.

More detailed information on preprocessing and analyses of fMRI data are reported in the Supplemental Material.