Log in

Development, Resilience Engineering, Degeneracy, and Cognitive Practices

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Drawing on a range of literature, I introduce two new concepts for understanding and exploring distributed cognition: resilience engineering and degeneracy. By re-examining Ed Hutchins’ (1995) ethnographic study of the navigation team I show how a focus on the developmental acquisition of cognitive practices can draw out several crucial insights that have been overlooked. Firstly, that the way in which agents learn and acquire cognitive practices enables a form of resilience engineering: the process by which the system is able to overcome and adapt to errors and the vagaries of nature. Secondly, that the best way to engineer a resilient system is through promoting degeneracy – how differing structures produce the same function – at the level of cognitive practices. These two features show that focusing on cognitive practices and developmental trajectories is important for both greater explanatory leverage; and is also useful in regard to the practicalities of designing real-world cognitive systems.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. See Heersmink (2015) for an explicit statement of this view.

  2. Degeneracy is a general term in biology that denotes structurally dissimilar processes that can perform the same function. The definition of degeneracy used in this paper, and discussed more in section 3, draws on the work of Paul Mason and others (Mason 2014; Mason et al. 2015). Mason details the history of this concept and the misunderstandings surrounding it that go beyond reaches of this paper.

  3. As of writing, Cognition in the Wild has been cited nearly fifteen-and-a-half thousand times according to Google Scholar.

  4. The regulations for the task specify ten individuals. It is a common feature of work that there is a gap between the prescriptive regulations and the actual work conditions (see Dejours et al. 2018; Deranty 2009, pp. 79–80). I consider this point in further detail below in section 3.2.

  5. It is important to note that although my focus here is solely on developmental trajectories, that I broadly agree with Fabry and others that cognitive historical factors are a crucial part of the explanatory picture for understanding distributed cognitive systems. Fabry (2017a), Hutchins (1995, 2006), Nersessian (2005), Menary (2014), and Sutton (2008, 2010) all have much to say about how historical timescales and how they impact on cognitive activity in differing ways. However, for reasons of space, I cannot detail these sufficiently here accept to acknowledge Kim Sterelny’s (2003) point that they have deep downstream impacts in engineering our environments epistemically and cognitively. I have indicated in the main text a few instances in which cognitive-historical features – what Hutchins refers to as “precomputations” – are involved in how the fix cycle is performed – e.g. the three-minute rule (see section 3.2 below). But this list is necessarily incomplete. Beyond historical timescales, several authors have forcefully argued that specific features of our evolutionary past – particularly our penchant for cultural-cognitive niche construction (Fabry 2018; Menary 2014; Sterelny 2003) – has had a profound impact on our cognitive profile, especially in terms of both our plasticity and our sociability (see also Henrich 2016; Heyes 2018).

  6. To be clear: although Hutchins does discuss cognition distributed in time, I think he fails to make it properly clear for the reader that this particular synchronic activity is dependent on diachronic factors. Additionally, although cognition distributed in time is a major component of his project throughout his work (1995, 2001, 2006, 2008, 2010a, 2010b), Hutchins does not delineate in a conceptually clear manner between differing timescales. In this instance, his work is not as clear as that of Fabry and others discussed in section 2.2.

  7. The original version of this example was conveyed to me by Greg Downey and is used here with permission.

  8. My thanks to two reviewers on drawing out this point in a more coherent fashion.

  9. There is degeneracy in terms of the structurally and functionally different kinds of representations between internal and external states – what Sutton (2010) refers to as the “complementarity thesis”.

  10. See Donald (1993) and Sutton (2010) for similar points about the importance of the differing cognitive properties of external and internal representations. Sutton labels this the “complementarity thesis”. The argument I present here is similar in kind and compatible with Sutton’s views but places an emphasis on the developmental aspects of acquiring the cognitive practices associated with learning to expertly manipulate these external representations.

References

  • Bietti, L.M., and J. Sutton. 2015. Interacting to remember at multiple timescales: Coordination, collaboration, cooperation and culture in joint remembering. Interaction Studies 16 (3): 419–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charbonneau, M. 2013. The cognitive life of mechanical molecular models. Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 44 (4): 585–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheon, H. 2014. Distributed Cognition in scientific contexts. Journal for General Philosophy of Science 45: 23–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., and D. Chalmers. 1998. The extended mind. Analysis 58 (1): 7–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davies, J., and K. Michaelian. 2016. Identifying and individuating cognitive systems: A task-based distributed cognition alternative to agent-based extended cognition. Cognitive Processing 17 (3): 307–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. 2007. Symbols and quantities in parietal cortex: Elements of a mathematical theory of number representation and manipulation. In Attention & Performance XXII. Sensori-motor foundations of higher cognition, ed. P. Haggard, Y. Rossetti, and M. Kawato, 527–574. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dejours, C., J.P. Deranty, E. Renault, and N.H. Smith. 2018. The return of work in critical theory: Self, society, politics. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deranty, J.P. 2009. What is work? Key insights from the psychodynamics of work. Thesis Eleven 98: 69–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. 1993. Origins of the modern mind – Three stages in the evolution of culture and Cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fabry, R.E. 2017a. Cognitive innovation, cumulative cultural evolution, and enculturation. Journal of Cognition and Culture 17 (7): 375–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fabry, R.E. 2017b. Explaining enculturated cognition. In Proceedings of the 39th annual conference of the cognitive science society, ed. E. Davelaar, G. Gunzelmann, A. Howes, and T. Tenbrink, 349–354. Austin: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fabry, R.E. 2018. Betwixt and between: The enculturated predictive processing approach to cognition. Synthese 195: 2483–2518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiore, S., C. Gabelica, T.J. Wiltshire, and D. Stokols. 2019. Training to be a (team) scientist. In Strategies for Team Science Success: Handbook of Evidence-Based Principles for Cross-Disciplinary Science and Practical Lessons Learned from Health Researchers, ed. K.L. Hall et al., 421–444. Zurich: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. 1980. Methodological solipsism considered as a research strategy in cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3: 63–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giere, R.N. 2006. Scientific Perspectivism. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Heersmink, R. 2015. Dimensions of integration in embedded and extended cognitive systems. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Science 14: 577–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. 2016. The secrets of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Oxford: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. 2018. Cognitive gadgets: The cultural evolution of thinking. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hollnagel, E., J. Pariès, D.D. Woods, and J. Wreathall, eds. 2011. Resilience engineering in practice. Surrey: Ashgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huebner, B. 2014. Macrocognition: A theory of Distributed minds and collective intentionality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. 1995. Cognition in the wild. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. 2001. Cognition, Distributed. In The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences, ed. R.A. Wilson and F.C. Keil, 2068–2072. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. 2005. Material anchors for conceptual blends. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 1555-1577.

  • Hutchins, E. 2006. The distributed cognition perspective on human interaction. In Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction, ed. N.J. Enfield and S.C. Levinson, 375–398. Oxford: Berg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. 2008. The role of cultural practices in the emergence of modern human intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 363: 2011–2019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. 2010a. Enaction, imagination, and insight. In Enaction: Towards a new paradigm cognitive science, ed. J. Stewart, O. Gapenne, and E.A. Di Paolo. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. 2010b. Imagining the cognitive life of things. In The cognitive life of things: Recasting the boundaries of the mind, ed. L. Malafouris and C. Renfrew, 91–102. Oxford: McDonald Institute Monograph.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ifrah, G. 1998. Universal History of Numbers: from prehistory to the invention of the computer. London: Harvill Press.

  • Kirchhoff, M.D. 2012. Extended cognition and fixed properties: Steps to a third-wave version of extended cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Science 11: 287–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsh, D., and P. Maglio. 1994. On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic actions. Cognitive Science 18: 513–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magnus, P.D. 2007. Distributed Cognition and the task of science. Social Studies of Science 37 (2): 297–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, P.H. 2014. Degeneracy: Demystifying and Destigmatizing a Core concept in systems biology. Complexity 20 (3): 12–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, P.H., J.F. Domínguez, B. Winter, and A. Grignoli. 2015. Hidden in plain view: degeneracy in complex systems. BioSystems 128: 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. 2007. Cognitive integration: Mind and Cognition unbounded. Hampshire: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. 2013a. Cognitive integration, enculturated cognition and the socially extended mind. Cognitive Systems Research 25-26: 26–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. 2013b. The Enculturated hand. In The Hand, an Organ of the Mind: What the Manual Tells the Mental, ed. Z. Radman, 349–367. London: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. 2014. Neural plasticity, neuronal recycling and niche construction. Mind and Language 29 (3): 286–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. 2015. Mathematical Cognition: A case of enculturation. In T. Metzinger & J. M. Windt (Eds.), Open MIND 25(T): 1–20. Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group.

  • Menary, R., and A.J. Gillett. 2017. Embodying culture: Integrated cognitive systems and cultural evolution. In The Routledge Handbook of Philosophy of the Social Mind, ed. J. Kiverstein, 72–87. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J.L., and T.R. Rocklin. 1998. The distribution of Distributed Cognition: Multiple interpretations and uses. Educational Psychology Review 10 (1): 97–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N.J. 2005. Interpreting scientific and engineering practices: Integrating the cognitive, social, and cultural dimensions. In Scientific and Technological Thinking, ed. M.E. Gorman, R.D. Tweney, D.C. Gooding, and A.P. Kincannon, 17–56. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N.J. 2006. Model-based reasoning in Distributed cognitive systems. Philosophy of Science 73 (5): 699–709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbet, R.E., I. Choi, K. Peng, and A. Norenzayan. 2001. Culture and Systems of Thought: Holistic versus analytic Cognition. Psychological Review 108 (2): 291–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D.A. 1986. Cognitive engineering. In User Centered System Design: New Perspectives in Human-Computer Interaction, ed. D.A. Norman and S. Draper, 31–61. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D.A. 1987. The psychology of everyday things. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. 1991. Cognitive artifacts. In Designing Interaction, ed. J.M. Carroll, 17–38. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poirier, P., and G. Chicoisne. 2006. A framework for thinking about distributed cognition. Pragmatics & Cognition 14 (2): 215–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roepstorff, A., J. Niewöhner, & S. Beck. 2010. Enculturing brains through patterned practices. Neural Networks 23, 1051–1059.

  • Rupert, R. 2011. Empirical arguments for group minds: A critical appraisal. Philosophy Compass 6 (9): 630–639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. 2003. Thought in a hostile world: The evolution of human Cognition. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, I. 2012. Seventeen equations that changed the world. London: Profile Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, J. 2008. Material agency, skills and history: Distributed Cognition and the archaeology of memory. In Material Agency: Towards a Non-Anthropocentric Approach, ed. C. Knappett and L. Malafouris, 37–55. London: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, J. 2010. Exograms and Interdisciplinarity: History, the extended mind, and the civilizing process. In The Extended Mind, ed. R. Menary, 189–226. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, Y., W. Zhang, C. Kewel, S. Feng, Y. Ji, J. Shen, E.M. Reiman, and Y. Llu. 2006. Arithmetic processing in the brain shaped by cultures. PNAS 103 (28): 10775–10780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Theiner, G., and T. O’Connor. 2010. The emergence of group Cognition. In Emergence in Science and Philosophy, ed. A. Corradini and T. O’Connor, 78–117. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vorms, M. 2012. Formats of representation in scientific theorizing. In Models, Simulations, and Representations, ed. P. Humphreys and C. Imbert, 250–272. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williamson, K., and J. Sutton. 2014. Embodied collaboration in small groups. In Brain Theory: Essays in Critical Neurophilosophy, ed. C.T. Wolfe, 107–133. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Woods, D.D., and M. Branlat. 2011. Basic Patterns in How Adaptive Systems Fail. In Resilience engineering in practice, ed. E. Hollnagel, J. Pariès, D.D. Woods, and J. Wreathall, 127–144. Surrey: Ashgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J. 1997. The nature of external representations. Cognitive Science 21 (2): 179–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J., and Norman. 1994. Representations in Distributed cognitive tasks. Cognitive Science 18: 87–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank both Richard Menary and Graham Thomas for useful comments on earlier drafts of this paper. Thanks to John Sutton, Albert Atkin, and Jean-Phillipe Deranty for discussions of some key ideas in the paper. Thanks to Emma Redfearn for drawing the diagrams. I would also like to thank two reviewers for their thoughtful criticisms and challenges.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alexander James Gillett.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gillett, A.J. Development, Resilience Engineering, Degeneracy, and Cognitive Practices. Rev.Phil.Psych. 13, 645–664 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00550-9

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00550-9

Keywords

Navigation