Log in

Does the COVID-19 pandemic change individuals’ risk preference?

  • Published:
Journal of Risk and Uncertainty Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this study we exploit not only regional but also age and gender variation in exposure to COVID-19 to investigate its impact on risk tolerance. This study is the first to use age and gender variation in exposure to COVID-19, whereas the identification in previous studies relied on regional variation or simple differencing before and after the pandemic. Using a nationally representative household panel survey in Japan, we find that individuals who were exposed to higher risk of COVID-19, measured by the mortality rate, became more risk-tolerant. The result is in line with findings whereby the experience of standout adverse events and the large resulting losses increase individuals’ risk tolerance. However, the effect appears diminished after the vaccine rollout. An analysis using detailed vaccine records indicates that the vaccination offset the increase in risk tolerance due to the mortality risk, especially for individuals with a higher mortality rate for COVID-19. While the literature on this topic has been inconclusive with a few studies reporting insignificant changes in risk tolerance after the pandemic, our result suggests that granular information on exposure to COVID-19 helps identify its impact.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price includes VAT (Brazil)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

All data supporting the findings of this paper are available upon request.

Notes

  1. Tsutsui and Tsutsui-Kimura (2022) provide the only study that uses Japanese data to examine changes “during” the pandemic using the sample period from March to June 2020.

  2. Adema et al. (2022) report different outcomes across experiments and self-reported risk tolerance. Although it is beyond the scope of this study, how risk tolerance is measured is important to explore (see also Schildberg-Horisch, 2018).

  3. The mean reservation price is somewhat lower after 2016. As we will discuss shortly, respondents to the JHPS-CPS varied over time, with a notable decline in the number of respondents in 2016, and thus changes in the sample may affect the sample mean.

References

  • Adema, J., Nikolka, T., Poutvaara, P., & Sunde, U. (2022). On the stability of risk preferences: Measurement matters. Economics Letters, 210, 110172.

  • Akesson, J., Ashworth-Hayes, S., Hahn, R., Metcalfe, R., & Rasooly, I. (2022). Fatalism, beliefs, and behaviors during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 64, 147–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Angrisani, M., Cipriani, M., Guarino, A., Kendall, R., & de Zarate Pina, J. O. (2020). Risk preferences at the time of COVID-19: An experiment with professional traders and students. Staff Reports 927, Federal Reserve Bank of New York.

  • Bu, D., Hanspal, T., Lao, Y., & Liu, Y. (2020). Risk taking during a global crisis: Evidence from Wuhan. CovidEconomics, 5, 106–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cramer, J. S., Hartog, J., Jonker, N., & Van Praag, M. (2002). Low risk aversion encourages the choice for entrepreneurship: An empirical test of a truism. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 48, 29–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drichoutis, A. C., & Nayga, R. M. (2022). On the stability of risk and time preferences amid the COVID-19 pandemic. Experimental Economics, 25, 759–794.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckel, C. C., El-Gamal, M. A., & Wilson, R. K. (2009). Risk loving after the storm: A Bayesian-Network study of Hurricane Katrina evacuees. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 69, 110–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eeckhoudt, L., Gollier, C., & Schlesinger, H. (1996). Changes in background risk and risk taking behavior. Econometrica, 64, 683–689.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrer, R., & Klein, W. M. (2015). Risk perceptions and health behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 55, 85–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frondel, M., Osberghaus, D., & Sommer, S. (2021). Corona and the stability of personal traits and preferences: Evidence from Germany. Ruhr Economic Papers, No. 903.

  • Gassmann, X., Malézieux, A., Spiegelman, E., & Tisserand, J.-C. (2022). Preferences after pan(dem)ics: Time and risk in the shadow of COVID-19. Judgement and Decision Making, 17, 745–767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerra, G., Zaimovic, A., Mascetti, G. G., Gardini, S., Zambelli, U., Timpano, M., Raggi, M. A., & Brambilla, F. (2001). Neuroendocrine responses to experimentally-induced psychological stress in healthy humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 26, 91–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollier, C., & Pratt, J. W. (1996). Risk vulnerability and the tempering effect of background risk. Econometrica, 64, 1109–1123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, R., & Allen, P. (2000). Democracy and fatalism in the former Soviet Union. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 2258–2574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graeber, D., Schmidt, U., Schroeder, C., & Seebauer, J. (2020). The effect of a major pandemic on risk preferences—Evidence from exposure to COVID-19. SSRN Working Paper, No. 3724461.

  • Hanaoka, C., Shigeoka, H., & Watanabe, Y. (2018). Do risk preferences change? Evidence from the Great East Japan earthquake. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 10, 298–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, G. W., Hofmeyr, A., Kincaid, H., Monroe, B., Ross, D., Schneider, M., & Swarthout, J. T. (2022). Subjective beliefs and economic preferences during the COVID-19 pandemic. Experimental Economics, 25, 795–823.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kash, K. M., & Dabney, M. K. (2001). Psychological aspects of cancer screening in high-risk populations. Medical and Pediatric Oncology, 36, 519–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lohmann, P., Gsottbauer, E., You, J., & Kontoleon, A. (2020). Social preferences and economic decision-making in the wake of COVID-19: Experimental evidence from China. SSRN Working Paper, No. 3705264.

  • Page, L., Savage, D. A., & Torgler, B. (2014). Variation in risk seeking behavior following large losses: A natural experiment. European Economic Review, 71, 121–131.

  • Quiggin, J. (1982). A theory of anticipated utility. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 3, 323–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quiggin, J. (2003). Background risk in generalized expected utility theory. Economic Theory, 22, 607–611.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schildberg-Horisch, H. (2018). Are risk preferences stable? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 32, 135–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shachat, J., Walker, M. J., & Wei, L. (2021a). How the onset of the Covid-19 pandemic impacted pro-social behaviour and individual preferences: Experimental evidence from China. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 190, 480–494.

  • Shachat, J., Walker, M. J., & Wei, L. (2021b). The impact of an epidemic: Experimental evidence on preference stability from Wuhan. AEA Papers and Proceedings, 111, 302–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, J., & Wu, S. (2011). Fatalism and savings. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 40, 645–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsutsui, Y., & Tsutsui-Kimura, I. (2022). How does risk preference change under the stress of COVID-19? Evidence from Japan. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 64, 191–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, P., & Palma, M. A. (2021). Do risk preferences change during COVID-19? Comparisons between Pre and On-Pandemic Parallel Experiments. SSRN Working Paper, No. 3849536.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank So Kubota for constructive comments. This research uses micro data obtained from the Preference Parameters Study of Osaka University’s 21st Century COE Program, “Behavioral Macro-Dynamics Based on Surveys and Experiments,” and its Global COE project, “Human Behavior and Socioeconomic Dynamics.” The views expressed in this paper are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the Ministry of Finance Japan, the IMF, its Executive Board, or IMF management.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tomohide Mineyama.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that there are no financial or non-financial interests that are directly or indirectly related to the work submitted for publication.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix A. Descriptive statistics

Appendix A. Descriptive statistics

Table 7 Descriptive statistics

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mineyama, T., Tokuoka, K. Does the COVID-19 pandemic change individuals’ risk preference?. J Risk Uncertain 68, 163–182 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11166-024-09427-5

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11166-024-09427-5

Keywords

JEL

Navigation