Log in

Meta-awareness, mind wandering and negative mood in the context of the continuity hypothesis of dreaming

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

This article has been updated

Abstract

In the waking state, in the absence of meta-awareness, mind wandering with specific contents can lead to negative mood. Such negative mood can be incorporated into dreaming according to the continuity hypothesis of dreaming. In this paper we argue that in the presence of what we call ‘sustained phenomenal meta-awareness’, negative mood would not follow mind wandering in waking. Sustained phenomenal meta-awareness has a non-sensory, non-affective phenomenal character. It is essentially intransitive, prereflectively self-aware, non-propositional, non-conceptual and devoid of subject-object structure. In other words, this unique kind of meta-awareness is non-representational. Evidence is then provided that such sustained phenomenal meta-awareness can be incorporated into the subsequent dream state as non-dual lucid dreaming in which, again, no negative mood would arise. Based on the latter observation, we have coined the term ‘mindful mind wandering’ and defined it as mind wandering in the presence of sustained phenomenal meta-awareness. We argue that not only does mindful mind wandering not lead to negative mood in waking, but also its incorporation into dreaming, as non-dual lucid dreaming, result in a state that is free of negative affection.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price includes VAT (United Kingdom)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

Not applicable.

Code Availability

Not applicable.

Change history

  • 05 August 2022

    Springer Nature’s version of this paper was updated to present the corrected Table 1.

Notes

  1. By ‘direction of the relationship’ we refer to the temporal priority between mind wandering and negative mood. However, temporal priority does not necessarily imply causality.

  2. This is not an uncommon approach toward the concepts of affect, emotion, and mood. Some scholars, such as Fox et al., (2018), Mason et al., (2013), and Mills et al., (2021), have adopted a similar stance in addressing the relationship between these concepts and mind wandering.

  3. The intensity and pervasiveness of waking emotions are among the most important factors that influence the incorporation of emotions into subsequent dreams. These factors can influence the time course of the incorporation of waking emotions into dreams so that not only can immediate pre-sleep emotions be incorporated into dreams, but also emotions experienced in a longer time interval (Schredl, 2018; Gilchrist et al., 2007; Hartmann, 2011b).

  4. In this article, we take ‘consciousness’ to be synonymous with ‘awareness’ and use the terms interchangeably where they are used as general terms. In this sense, they do not refer to any specific or ad hoc type of consciousness.

  5. By the statement ‘A-meta-awareness is a type of A-consciousness,’ we mean A-meta-awareness and A-consciousness share the same essential properties. It should be noted that any type of meta-awareness falls under the category of ‘consciousness’. Put another way, any higher order consciousness, such as A-meta-awareness, is still a type of consciousness.

  6. By the term ‘inaccessible’ in this context we mean that contents/processes of mind wandering cannot be accessed before my attendance toward them. As seen in what follows, it is my attendance that makes them available in the executive system. Otherwise, they are unavailable.

  7. In Tibetan Buddhism, this style of meditation is called ‘Mahāmudrā’.

  8. As opposed to ‘reflective self-awareness’ in which self-awareness requires conceptualization and higher-order representation of one’s ‘self’ as the subject of experiences.

  9. For further reading on the unitary, non-dual state of selflessness, see Farb et al., (2007); Siderits et al., (2013); Dahl et al., (2015); Dor-Ziderman et al., (2016); Metzinger (2020). Also note that some descriptions of the features of sustained P-meta-awareness, or non-dual awareness, will unavoidably seem paradoxical due to the feature of language which is inherently linear with dualistic subject-object structuring. Non-dual awareness, however, is inherently holistic (Josipovic, 2019). For example, while language demands for a statement to be expressed as ‘the meditator differentiates non-dual awareness from its objects,’ in such experiences there is no subject experiencing non-dual awareness as if it were an object differentiated from other objects. There is only awareness knowing its own being inherently and non-representationally.

  10. In Zen Buddhism, such an attitude toward mental experiences is called ‘Shoshin’ or ‘beginner’s mind’ (Suzuki, 1970). It refers to an attitude of openness and eagerness without having preconceptions.

  11. As we argue later in this section, such awareness can be dualistic and representational or non-dual without conceptualization or higher-order representation.

References

  • Abramovitch, H. (1995). The nightmare of returning home: A case of acute onset nightmare disorder treated by lucid dreaming. Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences, 32(2), 140–145

    Google Scholar 

  • Albahari, M. (2009). Witness-Consciousness: Its definition, appearance and reality. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 16(1), 62–84

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). American Psychiatric Pub

  • Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Kaiser, R. H., et al. (2013). A penny for your thoughts: Dimensions of self-generated thought content and relationships with individual differences in emotional wellbeing. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 900. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00900

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Appel, K., Pipa, G., & Dresler, M. (2018). Investigating consciousness in the sleep laboratory–an interdisciplinary perspective on lucid dreaming. Interdisciplinary Science Reviews, 43(2), 192–207. https://doi.org/10.1080/03080188.2017.1380468

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness: emotion regulation following a focused breathing induction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(12), 1849–1858. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.12.007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aviram, L., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2018). Lucid dreaming: Intensity, but not frequency, is inversely related to psychopathology. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 384. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00384

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baird, B., Mota-Rolim, S. A., & Dresler, M. (2019b). The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 100, 305–323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.03.008

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baird, B., Riedner, B. A., Boly, M., Davidson, R. J., & Tononi, G. (2019a). Increased lucid dream frequency in long-term meditators but not following mindfulness-based stress reduction training. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory Research and Practice, 6(1), 40–54. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000176

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (1995). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18(2), 227–247. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00038474

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (2002). Concepts of consciousness. In D. J. Chalmers (Ed.), Philosophy of mind: Contemporary readings (pp. 206–218). Oxford University Press

  • Braboszcz, C., Hahusseau, S., & Delorme, A. (2010). Meditation and neuroscience: From basic research to clinical practice. In R. Carlsted (Ed.), Integrative clinical psychology, psychiatry and behavioral medicine: Perspectives, practices and research (pp. 755–778). New York, NY: Springer

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandmeyer, T., & Delorme, A. (2020). Meditation and the wandering mind: A theoretical framework of underlying neurocognitive mechanisms. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620917340

  • Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell-Sills, L., Barlow, D. H., Brown, T. A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2006). Effects of suppression and acceptance on emotional responses of individuals with anxiety and mood disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(9), 1251–1263. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christoff, K., Gordon, A., & Smith, R. (2011). The role of spontaneous thought in human cognition. In O. Vartanian, & D. R. Mandel (Eds.), Contemporary topics in cognitive neuroscience series. Neuroscience of decision making (pp. 259–284). New York, NY, US: Psychology Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Creswell, J. D. (2016). Mindfulness interventions. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 491–516. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-042716-051139

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dahl, C. J., Lutz, A., & Davidson, R. J. (2015). Reconstructing and deconstructing the self: cognitive mechanisms in meditation practice. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(9), 515–523. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dainton, B. (2002). The gaze of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9(2), 31–48

    Google Scholar 

  • Dan-Glauser, E. S., & Gross, J. J. (2015). The temporal dynamics of emotional acceptance: Experience, expression, and physiology. Biological Psychology, 108, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.03.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dimidjian, S., & Segal, Z. V. (2015). Prospects for a clinical science of mindfulness-based intervention. American Psychologist, 70(7), 593–620. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039589

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, M. L., Girn, M., & Christoff, K. (2020). Brain network organization during mindful acceptance of emotions. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.03.31.018697

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domhoff, G. W. (2011). The neural substrate for dreaming: is it a subsystem of the default network? Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1163–1174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.03.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domhoff, G. W. (2017). The invasion of the concept snatchers: The origins, distortions, and future of the continuity hypothesis. Dreaming, 27(1), 14–39. https://doi.org/10.1037/drm0000047

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domhoff, G. W. (2018). The Emergence of dreaming: Mind-wandering, embodied simulation, and the default network. Oxford University Press

  • Dor-Ziderman, Y., Ataria, Y., Fulder, S., Goldstein, A., & Berkovich-Ohana, A. (2016). Self-specific processing in the meditating brain: a MEG neurophenomenology study. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2016(1), niw019. https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/niw019

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunne, J. (2011). Toward an understanding of non-dual mindfulness. Contemporary Buddhism, 12(1), 71–88. https://doi.org/10.1080/14639947.2011.564820

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunne, J. D. (2015). Buddhist styles of mindfulness: a heuristic approach. In B. D. Ostafm., M. D. Robinson., & B. P (Eds.), Handbook of mindfulness and self-regulation (pp. 251–270). New York: Springer

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dunne, J. D., Thompson, E., & Schooler, J. (2019). Mindful meta-awareness: Sustained and non-propositional. Current Opinion in Psychology, 28, 307–311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.07.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Esser, T. (2014). Kundalini and non-duality in the lucid dreaming state. In R. Hurd, & K. Bulkeley (Eds.), Lucid dreaming: New perspectives on consciousness in sleep 2 (pp. 233–263). Santa Barbara, California: Praeger

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, C. O. (1970). The Subject of Consciousness. London: G. Allen & Unwin

    Google Scholar 

  • Farb, N. A., Anderson, A. K., et al. (2010) Minding one’s emotions: Mindfulness training alters the neural expression of sadness. Emotion, 10(1), 25-33. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017151

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farb, N. A. S., Segal, Z. V., et al. (2007). Attending to the present: mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference. Social Cognitive and AffectiveNeuroscience, 2(4), 313-322.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsm030

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, K. C., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., et al. (2018). Affective neuroscience of self-generated thought. Ann NY Acad Sci, 1426, 25-51. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13740

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, K. C., & Christoff, K. (2014). Metacognitive facilitation of spontaneous thought processes: When metacognition helps the wandering mind find its way. In S. M. Fleming, & C. D. Fifth (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of metacognition (pp. 293–319). Berlin, Germany: Springer

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, K. C., Nijeboer, S., Solomonova, E., Domhoff, G. W., & Christoff, K. (2013). Dreaming as mind wandering: evidence from functional neuroimaging and first-person content reports. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 412. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00412

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, M. S., Mrazek, M. D., et al. (2013). The silver lining of a mind in the clouds: Interesting musings are associated with positive mood while mind-wandering. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 583. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00583

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fucci, E., Abdoun, O., et al. (2018). Differential effects of non-dual and focused attention meditations on the formation of automatic perceptual habits in expert practitioners. Neuropsychologia, 119, 92–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.07.025

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gackenbach, J. (1988). The psychological content of lucid versus nonlucid dreams. In J. Gackenbach, & S. LaBerge (Eds.), Conscious mind, slee** brain – Perspectives on lucid dreaming (pp. 181–220). New York, NY: Plenum Press

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gackenbach, J. (1991). A developmental model of consciousness in sleep: From sleep consciousness to pure consciousness. In J. Gackenbach, & A. Sheikh (Eds.), Dream images: A call to mental arms (pp. 287–308). New York, NY: Baywood

    Google Scholar 

  • Gackenbach, J., Cranson, R., & Alexander, C. (1986). Lucid dreaming, witnessing dreaming, and the transcendental meditation technique: A developmental relationship. Lucidity Letter, 5(2), 34–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Gamma, A., & Metzinger, T. (2021). The Minimal Phenomenal Experience questionnaire (MPE-92 M): Towards a phenomenological profile of “pure awareness” experiences in meditators. Plos One, 16(7), e0253694. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253694

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilchrist, S., Davidson, J., & Shakespeare-Finch, J. (2007). Dream emotions, waking emotions, personality characteristics and well-being–A positive psychology approach. Dreaming, 17(3), 172–185. https://doi.org/10.1037/1053-0797.17.3.172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillespie, G. (1988). Lucid dreams in Tibetan Buddhism. In J. Gackenbach, & S. LaBerge (Eds.), Conscious mind, slee** brain – Perspectives on lucid dreaming (pp. 27–35). New York, NY: Plenum Press

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. S., & Lind, R. E. (1970). Dreams, life, and literature: A study of Franz Kafka Chapel Hill. NC: University of North Carolina Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, E. (1991). Boundaries in the mind – A new psychology of personality. New York: Basic Books

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, E. (1998). Dreams and nightmares – A new theory on the origin and meaning of dreams. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Publishing

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, E. (2008). The Central Image (CI) makes “Big” dreams Big: The Central Image is the emotional heart of the dream. Dreaming, 18(1), 44–57. https://doi.org/10.1037/1053-0797.18.1.44

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, E. (2010). The underlying emotion and the dream: Relating dream imagery to the dreamer ‘s underlying emotion can help elucidate the nature of dreaming. International Review of Neurobiology, 92, 197–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0074-7742(10)92010-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, E. (2011a). Continuity? Yes, emotional continuity. International Journal of Dream Research, 4(2), 77. https://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2011.2.9154

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, E. (2011b). The nature and function of dreaming. New York: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasenkamp, W. (2018). Catching the wandering mind: Meditation as a window into spontaneous thought. In K. C. R. Fox, & K. Christoff (Eds.), The oxford handbook of spontaneous thought: Mind-wandering, creativity, and dreaming (pp. 956–980). New York: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasenkamp, W., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Duncan, E., & Barsalou, L. W. (2012). Mind wandering and attention during focused meditation: a fine-grained temporal analysis of fluctuating cognitive states. Neuroimage, 59(1), 750–760. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.008

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hattori, M. (1968). Dignaga, on perception. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, D. (2011). A reply to questions concerning mind and primordial knowing. Journal of the International Association of Buddhist Studies, 34, 31–96

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. H. (2007). Consciousness and self-awareness. Asian Philosophy, 17(3), 213–230. https://doi.org/10.1080/09552360701625460

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J. A., & Schredl, M. (2011). The continuity and discontinuity between waking and dreaming: A dialogue between Michael Schredl and Alan Hobson concerning the adequacy and completeness of these notions. International Journal of Dream Research, 4(1), 3–7. https://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2011.1.9087

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holzinger, B., Klösch, G., & Saletu, B. (2015). Studies with lucid dreaming as add-on therapy to Gestalt therapy. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 131(6), 355–363. https://doi.org/10.1111/ane.12362

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, H. (1987). Lucidity as a meditative state.Lucidity Letter, 6(2)

  • Hunt, H. T. (1991). Lucid dreaming as a meditative state: Some evidence from long-term meditators in relation to the cognitive-psychological bases of transpersonal phenomena. In J. I. Gackenbach, & A. A. Sheikh (Eds.), Dream images: A call to mental arms (pp. 265–285). Amityville, New York: Baywood

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, H. T., & Ogilvie, R. D. (1988). Lucid dreams in their natural series. In S. LaBerge, & J. Gackenbach (Eds.), Conscious mind, slee** brain (pp. 389–417). New York, NY: Plenum

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hurd, R., & Bulkeley, K. (2014). Lucid Dreaming: New perspectives on consciousness in sleep. Santa Barbara. California: Praeger

    Google Scholar 

  • Hymanjr, I., & Loftus, E. (1998). Errors in autobiographical memory. Clinical Psychology Review, 18, 933–947. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00041-5 PMID: 9885768

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1904). Does’ consciousness’ exist? The Journal of Philosophy Psychology and Scientific Methods, 1(18), 477–491. https://doi.org/10.2307/2011942

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jankowski, T., & Holas, P. (2014). Metacognitive model of mindfulness. Consciousness and Cognition, 28, 64–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.06.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Josipovic, Z. (2013). Freedom of the mind. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 538. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00538

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Josipovic, Z. (2014). Neural correlates of nondual awareness in meditation. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1307(1), 9–18. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12261

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Josipovic, Z. (2016). Love and compassion meditation: a nondual perspective. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1373(1), 65–71. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13078

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Josipovic, Z. (2019). Nondual awareness: consciousness-as-such as nonrepresentational reflexivity. In N. Srinivasan (Ed.), Progress in brain research: Meditation, Vol. 244. (pp. 273–298). Elsevier Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.10.021

  • Josipovic, Z. (2021). Implicit–explicit gradient of nondual awareness or consciousness as such. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2021(2), niab031. https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/niab031

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Josipovic, Z., & Miskovic, V. (2020). Nondual awareness and minimal phenomenal experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 2087. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02087

  • Kahan, T. L., & LaBerge, S. P. (2011). Dreaming and waking: similarities and differences revisited. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3), 494–514. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330, 932–932. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1192439

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kjellgren, A., & Taylor, S. (2008). Map** zazen meditation as a developmental process: Exploring the experiences of experienced and inexperienced meditators. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 40(2), 224–250

    Google Scholar 

  • Konjedi, S., & Maleeh, R. (2017). A closer look at the relationship between the default network, mind wandering, negative mood, and depression. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 17(4), 697–711. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-017-0506-z

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konjedi, S., & Maleeh, R. (2020). Sleep and mindfulness meditation as they relate to false memory. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 84(4), 1084–1111. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1098-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konjedi, S., & Maleeh, R. (2021). The dynamic framework of mind wandering revisited: How mindful meta-awareness affects mental states’ constraints. Consciousness and Cognition, 95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2021.103194

  • LaBerge, S. (1985). Lucid dreaming: The power of being awake and aware in your dreams. Los Angeles: Jeremy P. Tarcher

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, S. (2003). Lucid dreaming and the yoga of the dream state: A psychophysiological perspective. In B. A, & Wallace (Eds.), Buddhism & science: Breaking new ground (pp. 233–258). New York, NY: Columbia University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, S., & Rheingold, H. (1990). Exploring the world of lucid dreaming. New York: Ballantine Books

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, R., & Nielsen, T. (2009). Nightmares, bad dreams, and emotion dysregulation: A review and new neurocognitive model of dreaming. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(2), 84–88. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01614.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, E. K., & Creswell, J. D. (2017). Mechanisms of mindfulness training: Monitor and Acceptance Theory (MAT). Clinical Psychology Review, 51, 48–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.10.011

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, E. K., & Creswell, J. D. (2019). Mindfulness, acceptance, and emotion regulation: Perspectives from Monitor and Acceptance Theory (MAT). Current Opinion in Psychology, 28, 120–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.12.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lockwood, M. (1993). The grain problem. In H. Robinson (Ed.), Objections to physicalism (pp. 271–291). Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Meditation and the neuroscience of consciousness: An introduction. In P. D. Zelazo., M. Moscovitch., & E. Thompson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. 499–551). Cambridge University Press

  • Lutz, A., Jha, A. P., Dunne, J. D., & Saron, C. D. (2015). Investigating the phenomenological matrix of mindfulness-related practices from a neurocognitive perspective. American Psychologist, 70(7), 632–658. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039585

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 163–169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, J. M., & Troy, A. S. (2021). The role of nonduality in the relationship between flow states and well-being. Mindfulness, 12, 1639–1652. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-021-01627-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macedo, T. C. F., Ferreira, G. H., Almondes, K. M. D., Kirov, R., & Mota-Rolim, S. A. (2019). My dream, my rules: Can lucid dreaming treat nightmares? Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2618. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02618

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maleeh, R. (2009). The conscious mind revisited: An informational approach to the hard problem of consciousness. (Doctoral thesis, Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Osnabrück, Germany). Retrieved from https://repositorium.ub.uniosnabrueck.de/bitstream/urn:nbn:de:gbv:700-2009071511/2/E-Diss905_thesis. pdf

  • Maleeh, R. (2015). Minds, brains and programs: An information-theoretic approach. Mind and Matter, 13(1), 71–103

    Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski, J., & Horton, C. L. (2011). Themes of continuity: Commentary on “The continuity and discontinuity between waking and dreaming: A Dialogue between Michael Schredl and Allan Hobson concerning the adequacy and completeness of these notions”. International Journal of Dream Research, 4(2), 86–92. https://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2011.2.9149

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski, J., & Horton, C. L. (2014). Evidence for the preferential incorporation of emotional waking-life experiences into dreams. Dreaming, 24(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036017

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marchetti, I., Koster, E. H., & De Raedt, R. (2012). Mind wandering heightens the accessibility of negative relative to positive thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1517–1525. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.05.013

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maron-Katz, A., Ben-Simon, E., Sharon, H., Gruberger, M., & Cvetkovic, D. (2014). A neuroscientific perspective on meditation. In N. N. Singh (Ed.), Psychology of meditations (pp. 99–128). New York, NY: Nova Science

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, M., Brown, K., Mar, R. A., & Smallwood, J. (2013). Driver of discontent or escape vehicle: The affective consequences of mind wandering. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 477. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00477

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merritt, J. M., Stickgold, R., Pace-Schott, E., Williams, J., & Hobson, J. A. (1994). Emotion profiles in the dreams of men and women. Consciousness and Cognition, 3(1), 46–60. https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1994.1004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metzinger, T. (1995). Faster than thought. In T. Metzinger (Ed.), Conscious experience (pp. 425–460). Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic

    Google Scholar 

  • Metzinger, T. (2003). Being no one: The self-model theory of subjectivity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Metzinger, T. (2020). Minimal phenomenal experience: meditation, tonic alertness, and the phenomenology of “pure” consciousness. Philosophy and the Mind Sciences, 1(I), 1–44. https://doi.org/10.33735/phimisci.2020.I.46

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mills, C., Porter, A. R., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Christoff, K., & Colby, A. (2021). How task-unrelated and freely moving thought relate to affect: Evidence for dissociable patterns in everyday life. Emotion, 21(5), 1029–1040. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000849

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt, A., Hoffmann, R., et al. (1988). Dream psychology: Operating in the dark. In J. Gackenbach, & S. LaBerge (Eds.), Conscious mind, slee** brain (pp. 429–439). New York, NY: Plenum

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Mooneyham, B. W., Mrazek, M. D., Mrazek, A. J., & Schooler, J. W. (2016). Signal or noise: Brain network interactions underlying the experience and training of mindfulness. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1369(1), 240–256. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13044

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 638–662. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.638

  • Mota-Rolim, S. A., & Araujo, J. F. (2013). Neurobiology and clinical implications of lucid dreaming. Medical hypotheses, 81(5), 751–756. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2013.04.049

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mrazek, M. D., Phillips, D. T., Franklin, M. S., Broadway, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Young and restless: Validation of the Mind-Wandering Questionnaire (MWQ) reveals disruptive impact of mind-wandering for youth. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 560. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00560

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nir, Y., & Tononi, G. (2010). Dreaming and the brain: From phenomenology to neurophysiology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(2), 88–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norbu, N. (1987). The cycle of day and night. Barrytown, NY: Station Hill Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Norbu, N., & Katz, M. (1992). Dream yoga and the practice of natural light Ithaca. NY: Snow Lion Publications

    Google Scholar 

  • Norbu, N., & Katz, M. (2002). Dream yoga and the practice of natural light. Snow Lion Pub

  • Perkins, A. M., Arnone, D., Smallwood, J., & Mobbs, D. (2015). Thinking too much: Self-generated thought as the engine of neuroticism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 492–498. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perogamvros, L., Baird, B., et al. (2017). The phenomenal contents and neural correlates of spontaneous thoughts across wakefulness, NREM sleep, and REM sleep. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29(10), 1766–1777. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01155

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poerio, G. L., Totterdell, P., & Miles, E. (2013). Mind-wandering and negative mood: Does one thing really lead to another? Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 1412–1421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.09.012

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramm, B. J. (2019). Pure awareness experience. Inquiry, 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1080/0020174X.2019.1592704

  • Reed, H. (1978). Meditation and lucid dreaming: A statistical relationship. Sundance Community Dream Journal, 2, 237–238

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruby, F. J., Smallwood, J., Engen, H., & Singer, T. (2013). How self-generated thought shapes mood—The relation between mind wandering and mood depends on the socio-temporal content of thoughts. PloS One, 8(10), e77554. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0077554

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sahn, S. (1976). Drop** ashes on the Buddha. New York: Grove Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Schooler, J. W., Smallwood, J., et al. (2011). Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(7), 319–326. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2003). Continuity between waking and dreaming: A proposal for a mathematical model. Sleep and Hypnosis, 5, 38–52

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2006). Factors affecting the continuity between waking and dreaming: emotional intensity and emotional tone of the waking-life event. Sleep and Hypnosis, 8(1), 1

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2007). Personality correlates of flying dreams. Imagination Cognition and Personality, 27(2), 129–137. https://doi.org/10.2190/IC.27.2.d

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2018). Researching dreams: The fundamentals. Springer

  • Schredl, M., & Doll, E. (1998). Emotions in diary dreams. Consciousness and Cognition, 7(4), 634–646. https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1998.0356

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., & Reinhard, I. (2010). The continuity between waking mood and dream emotions: Direct and second-order effects. Imagination Cognition and Personality, 29(3), 271–282. https://doi.org/10.2190/IC.29.3.f

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Kane, M. J., et al. (2018). Mind-wandering as a natural kind: A family-resemblances view. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(6), 479–490. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.03.010

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sellars, W. (1956). Empiricism and the Philosophy of Mind. Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, 1(19), 253–329

    Google Scholar 

  • Siderits, M., Thompson, E., & Zahavi, D. (2013). Self, no self? Perspectives from analytical, phenomenological, and indian traditions. New York: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Sikka, P., Valli, K., Virta, T., & Revonsuo, A. (2014). I know how you felt last night, or do I? Self-and external ratings of emotions in REM sleep dreams. Consciousness and Cognition, 25, 51–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.01.011

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., & Andrews-Hanna, J. A. (2013). Not all minds that wander are lost: The importance of a balanced perspective on the mind wandering state. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 441. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00441

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., Fitzgerald, A., Miles, L. K., & Phillips, L. H. (2009). Shifting moods, wandering minds: Negative moods lead the mind to wander. Emotion, 9, 271–276. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014855

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparrow, G. S. (1987). Lucid dreaming: Dawning of the clear light. Virginia Beach, VA: ARE Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Sparrow, G. S., Thurston, M., & Carlson, R. (2013). Dream reliving and meditation as a way to enhance reflectiveness and constructive engagement in dreams. International Journal of Dream Research, 6(2), 84–93. https://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2013.2.10151

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spoormaker, V. I., & Van Den Bout, J. (2006). Lucid dreaming treatment for nightmares: a pilot study. Psychotherapy and psychosomatics, 75(6), 389–394. https://doi.org/10.1159/000095446

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., & Argembeau, D., A (2013). Concern induced negative affect is associated with the occurrence and content of mind-wandering. Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 442–448. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.01.012

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., Van Der Linden, M., & Argembeau, D., A (2012). Using the daydreaming frequency scale to investigate the relationships between mind-wandering, psychological well-being, and present-moment awareness. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 363. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00363

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stocks, A., Carr, M., et al. (2020). Dream lucidity is associated with positive waking mood. Consciousness and Cognition, 83, 102971. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2020.102971

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stumbrys, T. (2011). Lucid dreaming: Discontinuity or continuity in consciousness. International Journal of Dream Research, 4(2), 93–97. https://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2011.2.9146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stumbrys, T., & Erlacher, D. (2016). Applications of lucid dreams and their effects on the mood upon awakening. International Journal of Dream Research, 9, 146–150. https://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2016.2.33114

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stumbrys, T., Erlacher, D., & Malinowski, P. (2015). Meta-awareness during day and night: The relationship between mindfulness and lucid dreaming. Imagination Cognition and Personality, 34(4), 415–433. https://doi.org/10.1177/0276236615572594

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki, S. (1970). Zen mind, beginner’s mind. Shambhala Publications

  • Tang, Y. Y. (2017). The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation: How the body and mind work together to change our behavior. [Adobe Digital Editions version]. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-46322-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, Y. Y., Hölzel, B. K., & Posner, M. I. (2015). The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16, 213–225. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3916

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tanner, B. A. (2004). Multimodal behavioral treatment of nonrepetitive, treatment-resistant nightmares: A case report. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 99(3), 1139–1146. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.99.3f.1139-1146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teper, R., & Inzlicht, M. (2013). Meditation, mindfulness and executive control: The importance of emotional acceptance and brain-based performance monitoring. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 85–92. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss045

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, S., Pollak, M., & Kahan, T. L. (2015). Subjective qualities of dreams with and without awareness. Dreaming, 25(3), 173–189. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039242

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, E. (2015). Waking, dreaming, being: Self and consciousness in neuroscience, meditation, and philosophy. New York, NY: Columbia University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsongkhapa, L. D. (1996). Tsongkhapa’s six yogas of naropa. Ithaka, NY: Snow Lion

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J. (1996). Neurophenomenology: A methodological remedy for the hard problem. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 3(4), 330–349

    Google Scholar 

  • Voss, U., & Hobson, A. (2015). What is the state-of-the-art on lucid dreaming? – Recent advances and questions for future research. In T. Metzinger, & J. M. Windt (Eds.), Open MIND: 38(T). Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group. https://doi.org/10.15502/9783958570306

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Voss, U., Schermelleh-Engel, K., Windt, J., Frenzel, C., & Hobson, A. (2013). Measuring consciousness in dreams: the lucidity and consciousness in dreams scale. Consciousnessand Cognition, 22(1), 8-21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.11.001

    Google Scholar 

  • Voss, U., & Voss, G. (2014). A neurobiological model of lucid dreaming. In R. Hurd & K. Bulkeley (Eds), Lucid dreaming: New perspectives on consciousness in sleep (pp. 23-36). Santa Barbara, California: Praeger

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, B. A., & Hodel, B. (2012). Dreaming yourself awake: Lucid dreaming and Tibetan dream yoga for insight and transformation Boston. MA: Shambhala Publications

    Google Scholar 

  • Wangyal, T. R. (1998). The Tibetan yogas of dream and sleep. Ithaca, NY: Snow Lion

    Google Scholar 

  • Zadra, A., & Domhoff, G. W. (2011). Dream content: Quantitative findings. In M. H. Kryger, T. Roth, & W. C. Dement (Eds), Principles and practice of sleep medicine (5t ed., pp. 585- 594). Elsevier

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zadra, A. L., & Pihl, R. O. (1997). Lucid dreaming as a treatment for recurrent nightmares. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 66(1), 50-55. https://doi.org/10.1159/000289106

  • Zak, R. S., Auerbach, S. H., et al. (2010). Best practice guide for the treatment of nightmare disorder in adults. Journal of clinical sleep medicine, 6(4), 389-401. https://doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.27883

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

No funds, grants, or other support was received.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

These authors contributed equally to this work.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Reza Maleeh.

Ethics declarations

Conflicts of interest/Competing interests

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.

Ethics approval

Not applicable.

Consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Maleeh, R., Konjedi, S. Meta-awareness, mind wandering and negative mood in the context of the continuity hypothesis of dreaming. Phenom Cogn Sci 23, 105–131 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-022-09835-5

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-022-09835-5

Keywords

Navigation