Log in

Nudge to the future: capitalizing on illusory superiority bias to mitigate temporal discounting

  • Published:
Mind & Society Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Policymakers and institutions have developed an increasing interest in applying principles from cognitive science to encourage individuals to adopt behaviors, attitudes and perspectives that enable them to reach higher levels of personal and collective well-being. We focused on the value of nudging people to adopt a broader farsighted view when making their day-to-day decisions, overcoming the temporal discounting bias which leads them to prefer smaller immediate gains to larger future rewards. Following recent advances in the literature, we tried to mitigate this myopia promoting the connection that individuals feel between their present and future selves through a nudge which capitalizes on their natural tendency to illusory superiority. We defined and tested a paradigm to exploit persuasively this optimistic bias to mitigate temporal discounting through the mediation of an increased future self-continuity. Implications for well-being and public policies are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price includes VAT (France)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ameriks J, Caplin A, Leahy J, Tyler T (2007) Measuring self control problems. Am Econ Rev 97:966–972

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson C, Berdahl JL (2002) The experience of power: examining the effects of power on approach and inhibition tendencies. J Pers Soc Psychol 83:1362–1377

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson C, Galinsky AD (2006) Power, optimism, and risk-taking. Eur J Soc Psychol 36:511–536. doi:10.1002/ejsp.324

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ariely D, Wertenbroch K (2002) Procrastination, deadlines, and performance: self-control by precommitment. Psychol Sci 13(3):219–224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aron A, Aron EN, Smollan D (1992) Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J Pers Soc Psychol 63:596–612

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron RM, Kenny DA (1986) The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 51:1173–1182

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron RA, Markman GD (2000) Beyond social capital: how social skills can enhance entrepreneurs’ success. Acad Manag Exec 14(1):106–116

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartels DM, Rips LJ (2010) Psychological connectedness and intertemporal choice. J Exp Psychol 139:49–69

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartels DM, Urminsky O (2011) On intertemporal selfishness: the perceived instability of identity underlies impatient consumption. J Consum Res 38:182–198

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck AT (1967) Depression: clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. Hoeber Medical Division, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Benhabib J, Bisin A (2007) The distribution of wealth: intergenerational transmission and redistributive policies. Working Paper, New York University

  • Blumenthal-Barby JS, Burroughs H (2012) Seeking better health care outcomes: the ethics of using the “nudge”. Am J Bioeth 12(2):1–10. doi:10.1080/15265161.2011.634481

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buunk P (2001) Perceived superiority of one’s own relationship and perceived prevalence of happy and unhappy relationships. Br Psychol Soc 40:565–574. doi:10.1348/014466601164984

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper JC, Kovacic WE (2012) Behavioral economics: implications for regulatory behavior. J Regul Econ 41:41–58

  • Epstein S (1994a) Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. Am Psychol 49:709

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein S (1994b) Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. Am Psychol 49(8):709–724

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ersner-Hershfield H, Garton MT, Ballard K, Samanez-Larkin GR, Knutson B (2009a) Don’t stop thinking about tomorrow: individual differences in future self-continuity account for saving. Judgm Decis Mak 4:280–286

    Google Scholar 

  • Ersner-Hershfield H, Wimmer GE, Knutson B (2009b) Saving for the future self: neural measures of future self-continuity predict temporal discounting. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 4:85–92. doi:10.1093/scan/nsn042

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ersner-Hershfield HE, Goldstein DG, Sharpe WF, Fox J, Yeykelvis L, Carstensen LL, Bailenson J (2011) Increasing saving behavior through age-progressed renderings of the future self. J Mark Res 48:23–37

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fast NJ, Gruenfeld DH, Sivanathan N, Galinsky AD (2009) Illusory control: a generative force behind power’s farreaching effects. Psychol Sci 20:502–508. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02311.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fraser C, Hite RE, Sauer PL (1988) Increasing contributions in solicitation campaigns: the use of large and small anchorpoints. J Cosum Res 15(2):284–287

  • Frederick S, Loewenstein G, O’donoghue T (2002) Time discounting and time preference: a critical review. J Econ Lit 40(2):351–401

  • Galinsky AD, Gruenfeld DH, Magee JC (2003) From power to action. J Pers Soc Psychol 85:453–466

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gattig ALW (2002) Intertemporal decision making: studies on the working of myopia. Dissertation, University of Groningen

  • Gilovich T, Griffin D, Kahneman D (2002) Heuristics and biases: the psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge university Press, Cambridge

  • Guala F, Mittone L (2013) A political justification of nudging. Rev Philos Psychol. doi:10.1007/s13164-015-0241-8

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoorens V, Harris P (1998) Distortions in reports of health behaviors: the time span effect and illusory superiority. Psychol Health 13(3):451–466. doi:10.1080/08870449808407303

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson DD, Fowler JH (2011) The evolution of overconfidence. Nature 477(7364):317–320. doi:10.1038/nature10384

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson EJ, Goldstein D (2003) Do defaults save lives? Science 302(5649):1338–1339

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joshi PD, Fast NJ (2013) Power and reduced temporal discounting. Psychol Sci 24:432. doi:10.1177/0956797612457950

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby KN, Marakovic N (1996) Modeling myopic decisions: evidence for hyperbolic delay-discounting within subjects and amounts. Organ Behav Hum Decis 64:22–30. doi:10.1006/obhd.1995.1086

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laney C et al (2008) Asparagus, a love story: healthier eating could be just a false memory away. Exp Psychol 55(5):291–300

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larwood L, Whittaker W (1977) Managerial myopia: self-serving biases in organizational planning. J Appl Psychol 62(2):194–198

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linkenbach J, Perkins HW (2003) Most of us wear seatbelts: the process and outcomes of a 3-year statewide adult seatbelt campaign in Montana. Presented at the national conference on the social norms model, Boston, MA, July 17

  • May RM, Tofler GH, Bartrop R et al (2010) Smoking cessation through a novel behavior modification technique. Am J Cardiol 106(1):44–46

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magen E, Dweck CS, Gross JJ (2008) The hidden zero effect: representing a single choice as an extended sequence reduces impulsive choice. Psychol Sci 19(7):648–649

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews G, Davies DR, Westerman SJ, Stammers RB (2000) Human performance: cognition, stress, and individual differences. Psychology Press, London

  • Messick DM, Bloom S, Boldizar JP, Samuelson CD (1985) Why we are fairer than others. J Exp Soc Psychol 21(5):480–500. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(85)90031-9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez ML (1989) Delay of gratification in children. Science 244:933–938

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Odean T (1998) Do investors trade too much? Am Econ Rev 89:1279–1298

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parfit D (1971) Personal identity. Philos Rev 80:3–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parfit D (1984) Reasons and persons. Clarendon Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachlinski J, Farina CR (2002) Cognitive psychology and optimal governmental design. Cornell Law Rev 87:549–615

    Google Scholar 

  • Strunk DR, Lopez H, DeRubeis RJ (2006) Depressive symptoms are associated with unrealistic negative predictions of future life events. Behav Res Ther 44(6):861–882. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.07.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sunstein CR (2015) Nudges, agency, and abstraction: a reply to critics. Rev Philos Psychol 6(3):511–529

  • Svenson O (1981) Are we all less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers? Acta Psychol 47(2):143–148. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(81)90005-6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor SE, Brown JD (1988) Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychol Bull 103(2):193–210

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler RH, Shefrin HM (1981) An economic theory of self-control. J Polit Econ 89(2):392–406

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler R, Sunstein CR (2008) Nudge: improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. Yale University Press, New Haven

    Google Scholar 

  • Tiger L (1979) Optimism: the biology of hope. Simon and Schuster, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky A, Kahneman D (1974) Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science 185(4157):1124–1131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Volpp KG, Loewenstein G, Troxel AB et al (2008) A test of financial incentives to improve warfarin adherence. BMC Health Serv Res 8:272. doi:10.1186/1472-6963-8-272

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wansink B, van Ittersum K, Painter JE (2006) Ice cream illusions bowls, spoons, and self-served portion sizes. Am J Prev Med 31:240–243

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein ND (1980) Unrealistic optimism about future life events. J Pers Soc Psychol 39(5):806–820

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams BR, Bezner J, Chesbro SB, Leavitt R (2005) The effect of a behavioral contract on adherence to a walking program in postmenopausal African American women. J Geriatr Phys Ther 21(4):332–342

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamada M, Uddin L, Takahashi H et al (2013) Superiority illusion arises from resting-state brain networks modulated by dopamine. Proc Natl Acad Sci 110:4363–4367

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman W, Jost JT (2001) What Makes you think you’re so popular? Self-evaluation maintenance and the subjective side of the” friendship paradox”. Soc Psychol Q 64:207–223

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Davide Pietroni.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pietroni, D., Hughes, S.V. Nudge to the future: capitalizing on illusory superiority bias to mitigate temporal discounting. Mind Soc 15, 247–264 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299-016-0193-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299-016-0193-4

Keywords

Navigation