Log in

Writing motivation and performance in Chinese children

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the relationships between writing motivation and written composition performance in Chinese among 388 Chinese children in Grades 3 to 5 in accordance with the framework of self-determination theory. Multiple regression analysis results showed that writing motivation measures (external regulation, introjected regulation, identified regulation and intrinsic motivation) contributed a unique variance to written composition performance before and after controlling for the contribution of cognitive-linguistic skills important to written composition (i.e., word dictation, working memory and syntactic knowledge). Identified regulation was the only significant predictor of written composition performance. These results are discussed with reference to the impact of Chinese culture on motivation to write among Chinese children.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baker, L., & Wigfield, A. (1999). Dimensions of children’s motivation for reading and their relations to reading activity and reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly,34(4), 452–477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bao, X.-H., & Lam, S.-F. (2008). Who makes the choice? Rethinking the role of autonomy and relatedness in Chinese children’s motivation. Child Development,79(2), 269–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borgers, N., Hox, J., & Sikkel, D. (2004). Response effects in surveys on children and adolescents: The effect of number of response options, negative wording, and neutral mid-point. Quality & Quantity,38(1), 17–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, L., Mcbride-Chang, C., Stewart, S., & Au, E. (2003). Life satisfaction, self-concept, and family relations in Chinese adolescents and children. International Journal of Behavioral Development,27(2), 182–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, C. S., Stevenson, H. W., Hayward, C., & Burgess, S. (1995). Culture and achievement: Ethnic and cross-cultural differences. In M. L. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 9, pp. 119–151). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, C., & Pomerantz, E. (2012). Why does parents’ involvement enhance children’s achievement? The role of parent-oriented motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(3), 820–832.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chinese Language Education Section, Curriculum Development Institute, Education Bureau, the government of the HKSAR. (2007). Lexical Lists for Chinese Learning in Hong Kong. Retrieved from https://www.edbchinese.hk/lexlist_ch/.

  • Chow, S. S. Y., & Chu, M. H. T. (2007). The impact of filial piety and parental involvement on academic achievement motivation in Chinese secondary school students. Asian Journal of Counseling,14, 91–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Ailly, H. (2003). Children’s autonomy and perceived control in learning: A model of motivation and achievement in Taiwan. Journal of Educational Psychology,95(1), 84–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., Hodges, R., Pierson, L., & Tomassone, J. (1992). Autonomy and competence as motivational factors in students with learning disabilities and emotional handicaps. Journal of Learning Disabilities,25, 457–471.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The what and why of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry,11, 227–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez, A. L., & Wolters, C. A. (2009). The relation between perceived parenting practices and achievement motivation in mathematics. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 21, 203–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfried, A. E. (1990). Academic intrinsic motivation in young elementary school children. Journal of Educational Psychology,82(3), 525–538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grolnick, W. S., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1997). Internalization within the family: The self-determination theory perspective. In J. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and children’s internalization of values: A handbook of contemporary theory (pp. 135–161). New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Parent styles associated with children’s self-regulation and competence in school. Journal of Educational Psychology,81, 143–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grolnick, W. S., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1991). Inner resources for school achievement: Motivational mediators of children’s perceptions of their parents. Journal of Educational Psychology,83(4), 508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guan, C., Ye, F., Meng, W., & Leong, C. (2013). Are poor Chinese text comprehenders also poor in written composition? Annals of Dyslexia,63(3), 217–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guan, C. Q., Ye, F., Wagner, R. K., Meng, W., & Leong, C. K. (2014). Text comprehension mediates morphological awareness, syntactic processing, and working memory in predicting Chinese written composition performance. Journal of Educational Psychology,106(3), 779–798.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guay, F., Chanal, J., Ratelle, C. F., Marsh, H. W., Larose, S., & Boivin, M. (2010). Intrinsic, identified, and controlled types of motivation for school subjects in young elementary school children. British Journal of Educational Psychology,80(4), 711–735.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hau, K. T., & Ho, I. T. (2010). Chinese students’ motivation and achievement. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of Chinese psychology (pp. 187–204). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J. R. (1996). A new framework for understanding cognition and affect in writing. In C. M. Levy & S. Ransdell (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences, and applications (pp. 1–27). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J. R. (2012). Modeling and remodeling writing. Writing Communication,29(3), 369–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J. R., & Flower, L. S. (1980). Identifying the organization of writing processes. In L. W. Gregg & E. R. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing (pp. 3–30). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hernandez, M., & Iyengar, S. S. (2001). What drives whom? A cultural perspective on human agency. Social Cognition,19, 269–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hidi, S., & Boscolo, P. (2007). Writing and motivation. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiromori, T. (2003). What enhances language learners’ motivation?: High school English learners’ motivation from the perspective of self-determination theory. JALT Journal,25, 173–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, I. T., & Hau, K.-T. (2008). Academic achievement in the Chinese context: The role of goals, strategies, and effort. International Journal of Psychology,43, 892–897.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., Wong, Y.-K., Yeung, P.-S., Chan, D. W.-O., Chung, K. K.-H., Lo, S.-C., & Luan, H. (2012). The core components of reading instruction in Chinese. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25(4), 857–886.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hong, Y. (2001). Chinese students’ and teachers’ inferences of effort and ability. In F. Salili, C. Y. Chiu, & Y. Y. Hong (Eds.), Student motivation: The culture and context of learning (pp. 105–120). New York, NY: Kluwer Academic, Plenum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsiang, T. P., & Graham, S. (2016). Teaching writing in grades 4–6 in urban schools in the Greater China Region. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal,29(5), 869–902.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hui, E. K. P., Sun, R. C. F., Chow, S. S.-Y., & Chu, M. H.-T. (2011). Explaining Chinese students’ academic motivation: Filial piety and self-determination. Educational Psychology,31(3), 377–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Rethinking the value of choice: A cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,76, 349–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joussemet, M., Landry, R., & Koestner, R. (2008). A self-determination theory perspective on parenting. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 49, 194–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koestner, R., & Losier, G. F. (2002). Distinguishing three ways of being internally motivated: A closer look at introjection, identification, and intrinsic motivation. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of Self-determination Research (pp. 101–121). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lam, R. (2018). Testing, drilling and learning: What purpose does the Grade 3 Territory-wide System Assessment serve? Asia Pacific Education Review,19(3), 363–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lau, K.-L. (2009). Grade differences in reading motivation among Hong Kong primary and secondary students. British Journal of Educational Psychology,79, 713–733.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lau, K.-L., & Chan, D. W. (2003). Reading strategy use and motivation among Chinese good and poor readers in Hong Kong. Journal of Research in Reading,26, 177–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lau, K. L., & Chan, D. W. (2007). The effects of cognitive strategy instruction on Chinese reading comprehension among Hong Kong low achieving students. Reading and Writing,20, 833–857.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C., Tse, S., Loh, K., & Hau, K. (2008). Text comprehension in Chinese children: Relative contribution of verbal working memory, pseudoword reading, rapid automatized naming, and onset-rime phonological segmentation. Journal of Educational Psychology,100(1), 135–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R., Corpus, J. H., & Iyengar, S. S. (2005). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations in the classroom: Age differences and academic correlates. Journal of Educational Psychology,97, 184–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, J. (2002). A cultural model of learning: Chinese ‘‘heart and mind for wanting to learn’.’. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,33, 248–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, J. (2004). Learning as a task and a virtue: U.S. and Chinese preschoolers explain learning. Developmental Psychology,40, 595–605.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, J. (2005). Mind or virtue: Western and Chinese beliefs about learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science,14(4), 190–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, J., & Wang, Q. (2004). Perceptions of achievement and achieving peers in U.S. and Chinese Kindergartners. Social Development,13(3), 413–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, D., Wong, K. K., & McBride-Chang, C. (2012). Reading motivation and reading comprehension in Chinese and English among bilingual students. Reading and Writing,25, 717–737.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, S., Medford, E., & Hughes, N. (2011). The importance of intrinsic motivation for high and low ability readers’ reading comprehension performance. Learning and Individual Differences,21(1), 124–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markland, D., Richard, M. R., Tobin, V. J., & Rollnick, S. (2005). Motivational interviewing and self-determination theory. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,24(6), 811–831.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miserandino, M. (1996). Children who do well in school: Individual differences in perceived competence and autonomy in above-average children. Journal of Educational Psychology,88(2), 203–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moneta, G. B., & Siu, C. M. Y. (2002). Trait intrinsic and extrinsic motivations, academic performance, and creativity in Hong Kong college students. Journal of College Student Development,43, 664–682.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noels, K. A., Pelletier, L., Clément, R., & Vallerand, R. (2000). Why are you learning a second language? Motivational orientations and self-determination theory. Language Learning,50, 57–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • OECD programme For International Student Assessment. (2003). Literacy skills for the world of tomorrow further results from PISA 2000. Paris: OECD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patrick, B. C., Skinner, E. A., & Connell, J. P. (1993). What motivates children’s behavior and emotion? Joint effects of perceived control and autonomy in the academic domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,65(4), 781–791.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, E. M., Qin, L., Wang, Q., & Chen, H. (2011). Changes in early adolescents’ sense of responsibility to their parents in the United States and China: Implications for their academic functioning. Child Development,82, 1136–1151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative processes. Journal of Personality,63, 397–427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and internalization: Examining reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,57(5), 749–761.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000a). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology,25, 54–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R., & Deci, E. L. (2000b). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., Stiller, J. D., & Lynch, J. H. (1994). Representations of relationships to teachers, parents, and friends as predictors of academic motivation and self-esteem. Journal of Early Adolescence,14, 226–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salili, F. (1995). Explaining Chinese motivation and achievement. In M. L. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement: Culture, motivation, and achievement (pp. 73–118). Greenwich, CT: JAI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salili, F., Chiu, C. Y., & Lai, S. (2001). The influence of culture and context on students’ motivation orientation and performance. In F. Salili, C. Y. Chiu, & Y. Y. Hong (Eds.), Student motivation: The culture and context of learning (pp. 221–247). New York, NY: Kluwer Academic, Plenum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka, M. (2013). Examining Kanji learning motivation using self-determination theory. System: An International Journal of Educational Technology and Applied Linguistics,41(3), 804–816.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tang, J., Li, N., Sandoval, J., & Liu, R. (2018). Parenting styles and academic motivation: A sample from Chinese high schools. Journal of Child and Family Studies,27(10), 3395–3401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tao, V., & Hong, Y. (2014). When academic achievement is an obligation: Perspectives from social-oriented achievement motivation. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,45(1), 110–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tse, S.-K., & Hui, S.-Y. (2016). Chinese writing curriculum reforms in Hong Kong in recent years and their impact on teaching and learning. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal,29(5), 1013–1037.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tse, S. K., Loh, E. K. Y., Cheung, W. M., & Kwan, C. Y. (2005). The Hong Kong writing project: Writing reform in primary schools. In G. Rijlarrsdam, M. S. K. Shum, & D. L. Zhang (Eds.), Studies in writing book series (Vol. 16, pp. 171–197). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallerand, R. J. (2001). A hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in sport and exercise. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Advances in motivation in sport and exercise (pp. 263–319). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K., & Deci, E. (2004). Motivating learning, performance, and persistence: The synergistic effects of intrinsic goal contents and autonomy-supportive contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,87(2), 246–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, J. H., & Guthrie, J. T. (2004). Modeling the effects of intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, amount of reading, and past reading achievement on text comprehension between U.S. and Chinese students. Reading Research Quarterly,39(2), 162–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, M. W., & Coffey, D. Y. (2004). Reading motivation: Multidimensional and indeterminate. Journal of Educational Psychology,96(1), 110–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Way, N., Okazaki, S., Zhao, J., Kim, J., Chen, X., Yoshikawa, H., et al. (2013). Social and emotional parenting: Mothering in a changing Chinese society. Asian American Journal of Psychology,4(1), 61–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wigfield, A. (1997). Reading motivation: A domain-specific approach to motivation. Educational Psychologist,32, 59–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J. (1997). Relations of children’s motivation for reading to the amount and breadth of their reading. Journal of Educational Psychology,89(3), 420–432.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wong, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1991). Motivation and academic achievement: The effects of personality traits and the duality of experience. Journal of Personality,59(3), 539–574.

    Google Scholar 

  • Xu, Q. (2010). Family changes in contemporary China: A review of parenting and fatherhood. International Journal of Current Chinese Studies,1, 125–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yan, C., McBride-Chang, M., Wagner, W., Zhang, C., Wong, R., & Shu, K. (2012). Writing quality in Chinese children: Speed and fluency matter. Reading and Writing,25(7), 1499–1521.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, P.-S., Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W.-O., & Chung, K. K.-H. (2013a). Contribution of oral language skills, linguistic skills, and transcription skills to Chinese written composition among fourth-grade students. Discourse Processes, 50, 498–529.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, P.-S., Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W.-O., & Chung, K. K.-H. (2013b). Modeling the relationships between cognitive-linguistic skills and writing in Chinese among elementary grades students. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 26, 1195–1221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, P.-S., Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W.-O., & Chung, K. K.-H. (2014). What are the early indicators of persistent word reading difficulties among Chinese readers in elementary grades? Dyslexia, 20, 119–145. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1471

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, P.-S., Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W.-O., & Chung, K. K.-H. (2017a). A “Simple View of Writing” in Chinese. Reading Research Quarterly, 52, 333–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, P.-S., Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W.-O., & Chung, K. K.-H. (2017b). The role of transcription skills and oral language skills in Chinese writing among children in upper elementary grades. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38, 211–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, P.-S., Tsang, P. P.-Y., & Chan, S.-W. (2018, February). Effective instruction for struggling writers: A tiered-intervention model on Chinese writing. Paper presented at the Second Annual Conference of the Association for Reading and Writing in Asia, Tsukuba, Japan.

  • Yu, W.-M. (2015). Teacher leaders’ perceptions and practice of student assessment reform in Hong Kong: A case study. Planning and Changing,46, 175–192.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This project was funded by the Research Grants Council, General Research Fund, Project Codes: 17606215 and 17611119. Special thanks are due to Dr. Simpson Wai-lap Wong and Dr. Rebecca Wing Yi Cheng for their valuable advices and suggestions. The authors thank the teachers, parents, and students of the four participating schools for their support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Pui-sze Yeung.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix 1

An English translation of the questionnaire items for the four subtypes of writing motivation

1.

I do in-class written composition activities so that the teacher won’t yell at me. (EXT)

2.

I do in-class written composition activities because I want the teacher to think I’m a good student. (ITJ)

3.

I do in-class written composition activities because I want to learn to write better. (IDT)

4.

I do in-class written composition activities because I’ll feel bad about myself if they don’t get done. (ITJ)

5.

I do in-class written composition activities because they are fun. (INT)

6.

I do in-class written composition activities because that’s the rule. (EXT)

7.

I enjoy doing my in-class written composition activities. (INT)

8.

I try to write on difficult topics because I want the other kids to think I’m smart. (ITJ)

9.

I try to write on difficult topics because I’ll feel bad about myself if I don’t try. (ITJ)

10.

I try to write on difficult topics because it’s fun. (INT)

11.

I try to write on difficult topics because that’s what I am supposed to do. (EXT)

12.

I try to write on difficult topics to find out how good I am at writing. (IDT)

13.

I try to produce good compositions because that’s what I am supposed to do. (EXT)

14.

I try to produce good compositions so my teachers will think I’m a good student. (ITJ)

15.

I try to produce good compositions because I like doing a good job on my school work. (IDT)

16.

I try to produce good compositions because I will get in trouble if I don’t. (EXT)

17.

I try to produce good compositions because I’ll fell really bad about myself if I don’t do well. (ITJ)

  1. EXT—item for external regulation; ITJ—item for introjected regulation; IDT—item for identified regulation; INT—item for intrinsic motivation

Appendix 2

Rubrics for marking written composition task

I. Content

 0 score

Very poor—the responses include mostly irrelevant information

 1 score

The responses include information that is not relevant, slight off topic, and the expression is vague

 2 scores

The responses are relevant to the topic but the content is simple (such as repeating the same themes)

 3 scores

The responses are relevant to the topic. There is a description of the incidents/activities involved

 4 scores

The responses are relevant and have a clear focus. There is a detailed description of the incidents/activities involved

 5 scores

Very good—the responses are relevant, with a clear focus, detailed, creative and thoughtful

II. Vocabulary

 0 score

Very poor—the responses are made up of inappropriate choice of vocabulary

 1 score

The responses are made up of limited vocabulary and some misuse of words in written Chinese

 2 scores

The choice of vocabulary in written Chinese are generally appropriate, with some minor errors

 3 scores

The responses demonstrate rich choice of vocabulary. There is an appropriate usage of simple vocabulary and the responses demonstrate some appropriate uses of sophisticated vocabulary in written Chinese

 4 scores

The responses demonstrate appropriate use of rich and sophisticated choices of vocabulary. Able to write in written Chinese throughout the responses

 5 scores

Very good—the responses demonstrate rich, completely appropriate and sophisticated choice of vocabulary in written Chinese

III. Sentence structure

 0 score

Very poor—the responses are made up of incorrect sentence structures

 1 score

The responses are made up of incomplete sentence structures with many syntactic errors

 2 scores

The responses demonstrate some appropriate uses of simple sentence structures but some incomplete sentence structures are evident

 3 scores

The responses are good and demonstrate some appropriate uses of complex sentence structures (e.g., the use of connectives). Sentence flow is generally smooth

 4 scores

The responses are good and demonstrate appropriate use of complete sentences. Complex sentence structures (e.g., the use of connectives, metaphor) can also be found in the responses. Sentence flow is generally good

 5 scores

Very good—the responses demonstrate a wide range of sentence structure and appropriate use of subordinate clauses and complex sentences. Sentence flow is excellent

IV. Organization

 0 score

Very poor—the discourse is not organized and lacks logic

 1 score

The structure of the discourse is not clear. Incorrect paragraphing

 2 scores

The structure of the discourse is simple. The organization is somewhat loose and not coherent

 3 scores

The structure of the discourse is slightly loose. There is some inconsistency but the overall organization is coherent

 4 scores

The discourse is well organized and coherent, with a clear progression of ideas

 5 scores

Very good—discourse is well organized, coherent, and thoughtful with a logical and analytical progression of ideas

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Yeung, Ps., Ho, C.Sh., Chan, D.Wo. et al. Writing motivation and performance in Chinese children. Read Writ 33, 427–449 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09969-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09969-0

Keywords

Navigation