Log in

Development of spelling skills in a shallow orthography: the case of Italian language

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

    We’re sorry, something doesn't seem to be working properly.

    Please try refreshing the page. If that doesn't work, please contact support so we can address the problem.

Abstract

This study analyzed the spelling skills of Italian children as a function of school experience. We examined the writing performances of 465 first- to eighth-grade normal readers on a spelling test that included regular words, context-sensitive regular words, words with ambiguous transcription, and regular pseudowords. Based on the dual-route model (DRM), the regularity and orthographic complexity effects were considered to probe sublexical processing while the lexicality effect was taken to mark lexical processing. The analysis of spelling performances indicated that, among Italian children, both lexical and sublexical procedures are available since the first year of schooling. However, the two procedures showed different developmental trends. The DRM appears as a useful theoretical framework to describe the development of spelling in a relatively regular language such as Italian.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Here and following squared brackets indicate a phonological transcription, capital letters indicate the spelling of a stimulus and the asterisk marks non-lexical spelling errors.

  2. In the original paper, the authors referred to a graphemic layer and not to a graphemic buffer, although the graphemic layer served the buffering function of the graphemic buffer.

  3. Analog pseudowords were created modifying the initial consonant letter of high frequency words. Thus, they had the same rhyme from which they were derived. For non-analog pseudowords, other letters were also modified in such a way to make it impossible to identify the word from which they were derived.

  4. Italian words with ambiguous transcription are both words with common and uncommon sound-to-spelling map**. For instance, among the words with the phonetic group [kw], more frequent are the words in which the transcription is CUO (58%), less frequent those with the transcription QUO (37%), and rare those in which the phonetic group [kw] is realized by the string CQU (only 5%) (De Mauro, 2000). The same applies for words containing the syllables [t∫e], [∫e], [dʒe]: the transcriptions SCE, CE and GE are more frequent than SCIE, CIE and GIE.

References

  • Angelelli, P., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., Zoccolotti, P., & Luzzatti, C. (2004). Characteristic of writing disorders in Italian dyslexic children. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 17, 18–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Angelelli, P., Notarnicola, A., Costabile, D., Marinelli, C. V., Judica, A., Zoccolotti, P., et al. (2008). DDO Diagnosi dei disturbi ortografici in età evolutiva (Diagnosis of orthographic deficits in childhood). Trento: Erickson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Angelelli, P., Notarnicola, A., Judica, A., Zoccolotti, P., & Luzzatti, C. (2010). Spelling impairment in Italian dyslexic children: Does the phenomenology change with age? Cortex, 46, 1299–1311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ardila, A. (1991). Errors resembling semantic paralexias in Spanish-speaking aphasics. Brain and Language, 41, 437–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., & Pinzòn, O. (1989). Alexia and agraphia in Spanish speakers: CAT correlations and interlinguistic analysis. In A. Ardila & F. Ostrosky-Solis (Eds.), Brain organization of language and cognitive processes (pp. 147–230). New York, NY: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Barca, L., Burani, C., & Arduino, L. S. (2002). Word naming times and psycholinguistic norms for italian nouns. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 34, 424–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barry, C. (1988). Modelling assembled spelling: Convergence of data from normal subjects and “surface” dysgraphia. Cortex, 24, 339–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barry, C. (1994). Spelling routes (or roots or rutes). In G. D. A. Brown & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of spelling: Theory, process and intervention (pp. 27–49). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barry, C., & De Bastiani, P. (1997). Lexical priming of nonword spelling in the regular orthography of Italian. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 9, 499–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Burani, C., D’Amico, S., & Barca, L. (2001). Word reading and picture naming in Italian. Memory and Cognition, 29, 986–999.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bortolini, U., Tagliavini, C., & Zampolli, A. (1972). Lessico di frequenza della lingua Italiana Contemporanea (Word frequency lexicon of italian contemporary language). Milano: Garzanti.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brizzolara, D., Chiosi, A. M., Cipriani, P., & De Pasquale, L. (1993). L’apprendimento del linguaggio scritto in bambini con difficoltà di linguaggio orale: continuità o discontinuità? (Written language acquisition in children with oral language impairment: Continuity or discontinuity?). In G. Masi & A. Martini (Eds.), Apprendimento e patologia neuropsichica nei primi anni di scuola (Learning processes and neuropsychic pathology in the first years of schooling) (pp. 124–135). Roma: Borla.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. D. A., & Loosemore, R. P. W. (1994). Computational approaches to normal and impaired spelling. In G. D. A. Brown & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of spelling: Theory, process and intervention (pp. 319–336). UK. John Wiley: Chichester.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullinaria, J. A. (1994). Connectionist modelling of spelling. In Proceedings of the sixteenth annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 78–83). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

  • Campbell, R. (1983). Writing nonwords to dictation. Brain and Language, 19, 153–1778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M. (2004). Spelling development in alphabetic writing systems: A cross-linguistic perspective. European Psychologist, 9, 3–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., & Bruck, M. (1993). The effect of oral and written language input on children’s phonological awareness: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. (2001). The foundations of spelling ability: Evidence from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 751–774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castles, A., Holmes, V. M., & Wong, M. (1997). Variations in spelling style among lexical and sublexical readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 98–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (2000). Deep dyslexia is right-hemisphere reading. Brain and Language, 71, 299–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Ziegler, J., & Langdon, R. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cornoldi, C., & Colpo, G. (1998). Prove di Lettura MT. Manuale (The MT reading test. Manual of use). Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cossu, G., Gugliotta, M., & Marshall, J. C. (1995). Acquisition of reading and written spelling in a transparent orthography: Two non parallel processes? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 9–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuetos, F. (1993). Writing processes in a shallow orthography. Reading and Writing, 5, 17–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Mauro, T. (2000). Il dizionario della lingua italiana. (The dictionary of Italian language). Torino: Paravia. http://www.demauroparavia.it/.

  • Ehri, L. C. (1998). Grapheme-phoneme knowledge is essential for learning to read words in English. In J. L. Metsala & L. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 3–40). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernandes, S., Ventura, P., Querido, L., & Morais, J. (2008). Reading and spelling acquisition in European Portuguese: A preliminary study. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 805–821.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folk, J. R., & Jones, A. C. (2004). The purpose of lexical/sublexical interaction during spelling: Further evidence from dysgraphia and articulatory suppression. Neurocase, 10(1), 65–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folk, J. R., & Rapp, B. (2004). Interaction of lexical and sublexical information in spelling: Evidence from nonword priming. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 565–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folk, J. R., Rapp, B., & Goldrick, M. (2002). The interaction of lexical and sublexical information in spelling: What’s the point? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19(7), 653–671.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. E. Patterson, J. C. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301–330). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R., Katz, L., & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographical depth: A multilingual comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 104–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, N. L., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (1997). Progressive dysgraphia: Co-occurrence of central and peripheral impairments. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 975–1005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, N. L., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2000). The impact of semantic memory impairment on spelling: Evidence from semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia, 38, 143–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hillis, A. E., & Caramazza, A. (1991). Mechanisms for accessing lexical representations for output: Evidence from a category-specific semantic deficit. Brain and Language, 40, 106–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iribarren, I. C., Jarema, G., & Lecours, A. R. (2001). Two different dysgraphic syndromes in a regular orthography, Spanish. Brain and Language, 77, 166–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kreiner, D. S. (1992). Reaction time measures of spelling: Testing a two-strategy model of skilled spelling. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8, 765–776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laiacona, M., Capitani, E., Zonca, G., Scola, I., Saletta, P., & Luzzatti, C. (2009). Integration of lexical and sublexical processing in the spelling of regular words: A multiple single-case study in Italian dysgraphic patients. Cortex, 45, 804–815.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laiacona, M., Zonca, G., Scola, I., Capitani, E., Saletta, P., & Luzzatti, C. (2004). Integration of lexical and sublexical processing in the spelling of regular words: A multiple single-case study of dysgraphia in Italian patients. Brain and Language, 91, 90–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luzzatti, C., Laiacona, M., Allamano, N., De Tanti, A., & Inzaghi, M. G. (1998). Writing disorders in Italian aphasic patients: A multiple single-case study of dysgraphia in a language with shallow orthography. Brain, 121, 1721–1734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luzzatti, C., Laiacona, M., Allamano, N., De Tanti, A., Inzaghi, M. G., & Lorenzi, L. (1994). Un test per la diagnosi del deficit di scrittura: Principi di costruzione e dati normativi (An Italian test for the diagnosis of acquired writing disorders: Guidelines and normative data). Ricerche di Psicologia, 18, 137–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcolini, S., & Burani, C. (2003). Sviluppo della lettura morfo-lessicale in una lingua a ortografia regolare (Development of morfo-lexical reading in a shallow orthography). In G. T. Scalisi, M. Orsolini, & C. Maronato (Eds.), Bambini in difficoltà nell’apprendimento della lingua scritta (Children with learning disabilities of written language) (pp. 24–38). Roma: Kappa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcolini, S., Burani, C., & Colombo, L. (2008). Lexical effects on children’s pseudoword reading in a transparent orthography. Reading and Writing, 22(5), 531–544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marconi, L., Ott, M., Pesenti, E., Ratti, D., & Tavella, M. (1993). Lessico Elementare. Dati statistici sull’italiano letto e scritto dai bambini delle elementari (Elementary lexicon: Statistical data on Italian as written and read by elementary school children). Bologna: Zanichelli.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, G., Morton, F., Welch, V., & Desberg, P. (1980). The development strategies in spelling. In U. Frith (Ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martini, A., Brizzolata, D., Pecini, C., Dinetti, D., & Negrin, S. (2002). Aspetti diagnostici e riabilitativi in bambini dislessici italiani (Diagnosis and rehabilitation features of Italian dyslexic children). In I. Arcolini & G. Zardini (Eds.), I disturbi di apprendimento della lettura e della scrittura (Reading and spelling learning disabilities) (pp. 223–235). Milano: Angeli.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maschietto, D., & Vio, C. (1998). Acquisizione delle competenze alfabetiche ed ortografiche di lettura e di scrittura: proposte per un modello evolutivo (Acquisition of alphabetic and orthographic abilities in reading and spelling: Proposal of a developmental model). Psicologia clinica dello sviluppo, 1, 51–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazzotta, S., Barca, L., Marcolini, S., Stella, G., & Burani, C. (2005). Frequenza, immaginabilità ed età di acquisizione delle parole: In che misura influenzano la lettura dei bambini italiani? (Word frequency, imageability and age-of-acquisition: How do they influence Italian children’s reading?). Psicologica clinica dello sviluppo, 2, 247–266.

  • Morton, J. (1989). An information-processing account of reading acquisition. In A. M. Galaburda (Ed.), From reading to neurons (pp. 43–66). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, A., & Caramazza, A. (1994). Representation and connectionist models: The NET spell experience. In G. D. A. Brown & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of spelling: Theory, process and intervention (pp. 337–364). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orsolini, M., Fanari, R., Tosi, V., De Nigris, B., & Carrieri, R. (2006). From phonological recoding to lexical reading: A longitudinal study on reading development in Italian. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 576–607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K. E. (1986). Lexical but nonsemantic spelling. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 341–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In P. Gough, L. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), reading acquisition (pp. 107–143). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., & Coltheart, M. (2002). A dissociation between orthographic awareness and spelling production. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 43–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rapp, B., Epstein, C., & Tainturier, M. J. (2002). The integration of information across lexical and sublexical processes in spelling. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C. (1996). CPM Coloured Progressive Matrices (Serie A. Ab, B). Manuale (CPM Coloured progressive matrices (Series A, Ab, B). Manual of Use). Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rinaldi, P., & Burani, C. (2005). L’esplosione del vocabolario negli scritti dei bambini di scuola elementare (The vocabulary explosion in the written production of elementary school children). Rivista di Psicolinguistica Applicata, 5, 31–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roeltgen, D. P., Rothi, L. G., & Heilman, K. M. (1986). Linguistic semantic agraphia: A dissociation of the lexical spelling system from semantics. Brain and Language, 27, 257–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Romani, C., Di Betta, A. M., Tsouknida, E., & Olson, A. (2008). Lexical and nonlexical processing in developmental dyslexia: A case for different resources and different impairments. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 798–830.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Romani, C., Olson, A., Ward, J., & Ercolani, M. G. (2002). Formal lexical paragraphias in a single case study: How “masterpiece” can become “misterpieman” and “curiosity” “suretoy”. Brain and Language, 83, 300–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., & McGregor, J. (1984). Developmental dyslexia: A cognitive experimental analysis of phonological, morphemic and visual impairments. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1, 43–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2003). Reading acquisition: Cross linguistic data. In T. Nunes & P. Bryant (Eds.), Handbook of children’s literacy (pp. 43–65). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., & Serniclaes, W. (2006). Reading acquisition and developmental dyslexia. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L. S., Béchennec, D., & Serniclaes, W. (2003). Development of phonological and orthographic processing in reading aloud, in silent reading, and in spelling: A four-year longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 194–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, M., & Coltheart, M. (1988). Does reading develop in a sequence of stages? Cognition, 30, 139–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tainturier, M. J., & Rapp, B. (2000). The spelling process. In B. Rapp (Ed.), What deficits reveal about the human mind: A handbook of cognitive neuropsychology (pp. 263–289). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tainturier, M. J., & Rapp, B. (2001). The spelling process. In B. Rapp (Ed.), The handbook of cognitive neuropsychology: What deficits reveal about the human mind (pp. 263–289). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (1984). Individual differences among children in reading and spelling styles. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 463–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tressoldi, P. E. (1996). L’evoluzione della lettura e della scrittura dalla 2a elementare alla 3a media. Dati per un modello di sviluppo e per la diagnosi dei disturbi specifici (The development of reading and spelling abilities from 2nd to 8th grade. Data for a developmental model and for the diagnosis of specific learning disorders). Età evolutiva, 56, 43–55.

  • Wimmer, H., & Landerl, K. (1997). How learning to spell German differs from leaning to spell English. In C. A. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell: Research, theory and practice across languages (pp. 81–96). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zoccolotti, P., Angelelli, P., Judica, A., & Luzzatti, C. (2006). I disturbi evolutivi di lettura e scrittura (Reading and spelling developmental learning disabilities). Roma: Carocci Faber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Di Filippo, G., Judica, A., & Martelli, M. (2009). Reading development in an orthographically regular language: Effects of length, frequency, lexicality and global processing ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 1053–1079.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The analysis of the interaction between lexical and sublexical processes was carried out during a period of stay of A.N. in the laboratory of Dr. Johannes C. Ziegler. We thank Dr. Ziegler for his helpful advices on this matter.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paola Angelelli.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 5.

Table 5 De-randomized list of words with ambiguous transcription

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Notarnicola, A., Angelelli, P., Judica, A. et al. Development of spelling skills in a shallow orthography: the case of Italian language. Read Writ 25, 1171–1194 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9312-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9312-0

Keywords

Navigation