Log in

What the <0.70, 1.17, 0.99, 1.07> is a Symbol?

  • Published:
Minds and Machines Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The notion of a ‘symbol’ plays an important role in the disciplines of Philosophy, Psychology, Computer Science, and Cognitive Science. However, there is comparatively little agreement on how this notion is to be understood, either between disciplines, or even within particular disciplines. This paper does not attempt to defend some putatively ‘correct’ version of the concept of a ‘symbol.’ Rather, some terminological conventions are suggested, some constraints are proposed and a taxonomy of the kinds of issue that give rise to disagreement is articulated. The goal here is to provide something like a ‘geography’ of the various notions of ‘symbol’ that have appeared in the various literatures, so as to highlight the key issues and to permit the focusing of attention upon the important dimensions. In particular, the relationship between ‘tokens’ and ‘symbols’ is addressed. The issue of designation is discussed in some detail. The distinction between simple and complex symbols is clarified and an apparently necessary condition for a system to be potentially symbol, or token bearing, is introduced.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anonymous. (2006). Message board post found at http://www.eham.net/forums/CW/2800?ehamsid=26cffd80c22c01a4782ac76acd78aa1a, retrieved 17th November 2006.

  • Bechtel, W., & Abrahamsen, A. (2002). Connectionism and the mind (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Blackwells.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkeley, I. (2000). What the #$*%! is a Subsymbol? Minds and Machines, 10, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berkeley, I. (2006). Moving the goal posts: A reply to Dawson and Piercey. Minds and Machines, 16, 471–478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berkeley, I., Dawson, M., Medler, D., Schopflocher, D., & Hornsby, L. (1995). Density plots of hidden unit activations reveal interpretable bands. Connection Science, 7, 167–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bever, T., Fodor, J., & Garrett, M. (1968). A formal limitation of associationism. In T. Dixon & D. Horton (Eds.), Verbal behavior and general behavior theory, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, P. (1989). The neurocomputational perspective. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (1992). The presence of a symbol. Connection Science, 4, 193–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2006). Mental symbols. Philosophical Psychology, 19, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, R. (1989). Meaning and mental representation. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, M. (1998). Understanding cognitive science. Oxford, UK: Blackwells.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, M. (2004). Minds and machines: Connectionism and psychological modeling. Oxford, UK: Blackwells.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, M., & Piercey, C. (2001). On the subsymbolic nature of a PDP architecture that uses a non-monotonic activation function. Minds and Machines, 11, 197–218.

    Article  MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. (1997). The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egan, F. (1995). Computation and content. The Philosophical Review, 104, 181–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1975). The language of thought. Harvard, Mass.: Harvard U. P.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1980). Methodological solipsism considered as a research strategy in cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 63–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1998). Do You think in Mentalese: Remarks on some arguments of Peter Carruthers. In Critical condition: Polemical essays on cognitive science and the philosophy of mind (pp. 63–74). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

  • Fodor, J., & Pylyshyn, Z. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive architecture. Cognition, 28, 3–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hadley, R. (1994). Systematicity in connectionist language learning. Mind and Language, 9, 247–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hadley, R. (1997). Cognition, systematicity, and nomic necessity. Mind and Language, 12, 137–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hardcastle, V. (1996). Computationalism. Synthese, 105, 303–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haugeland, J. (1985). Artificial intelligence: The very idea. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haugeland, J. (1996). “What is Mind Design?”, in Haugeland (1997: pp. 1–28).

  • Haugeland, J. (1997). Mind design II. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, P., Berkeley, I., Bringsjord, S., Hardcastle, V., McKee, G., & Shufflebeam, R. (1997), What is a computer? An electronic discussion. The Monist, 80, 389–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopcroft, J., & Ullman, J. (1979). Introduction to automata theory, Languages, and computation. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, R. (1999). The nothing that is: A natural history of zero. Oxford, UK: Oxford UP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, J. (1689/1975). An essay concerning the human understanding (Ed. P. Nidditch). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.

  • Lumsden, D. (2002). Crossing the symbolic threshold: A critical review of terrence Deacon’s The Symbolic Species. Philosophical Psychology, 15, 155–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. (2001). The algebraic mind: Integrating connectionism and cognitive science. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mates, B. (1972). Elementary logic (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford U.P.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. (1980). Physical symbol systems. Cognitive Science, 4, 135–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. (1976). Computer science as empirical inquiry: Symbols and search. Communications of the Association for Computing Machinery, 19, 113–126.

    MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • O’Grady, W., Archibald, J., Aronoff, M., & Rees-Miller, J. (Eds.). (2005). Contemporary linguistics: An introduction (5th ed.). Boston: Bedford/St. Martin’s.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pezzulo, G., & Castelfranchi, C. (2007). The symbol detachment problem. Cognitive Processing, 8, 115–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pomerleau, D. (1993). Neural network perception for mobile robot guidance. Boston: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Port, R., & Leary, A. (2005). Against formal phonology. Language, 81, 927–964.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. (1984). Computation and cognition: Toward a foundation of cognitive science. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1992). The rediscovery of the mind. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silberberg, M. (2007). Principles of general chemistry. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smolensky, P. (1988). On the proper treatment of connectionism. The Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 11, 1–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turing, A. (1936). On computable numbers with an application to the Entscheidungs-Problem. In Proceedings of The London Mathematical Society, Vol. 2, pp. 230–265.

  • Vera, A., & Simon, H. (1994). Reply to Touretzky and Pomerleau: Reconstructing physical symbol systems. Cognitive Science, 18, 355–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgment

Thanks to the late James Patrick Dugal for the Morse Code example that appears in Section “Designation and Constraints.”

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Istvan S. N. Berkeley.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Berkeley, I.S.N. What the <0.70, 1.17, 0.99, 1.07> is a Symbol?. Minds & Machines 18, 93–105 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11023-007-9086-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11023-007-9086-y

Keywords

Navigation