Log in

Contingency and Behavior Analysis

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The concept of contingency is central to theoretical discussions of learned behavior and in the application of learning research to problems of social significance. This paper reviews three aspects of the contingency concept as it has been developed by behavior analysts. The first is the empirical analysis of contingency through experimental studies of both human and nonhuman behavior. The second is the synthesis of experimental studies in theoretical and conceptual frameworks to yield a more general account of contingency and to integrate the concept with other behavioral processes. The third aspect is one of practical considerations in the application of the contingency concept in both laboratory and applied settings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price includes VAT (United Kingdom)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anger, D. (1956). The dependence of interresponse times upon the relative reinforcement of different interresponse times. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52, 145–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arbuckle, J. L., & Lattal, K. A. (1988). Changes in functional response units with briefly delayed reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, 249–263.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Azrin, N. H. (1956). Effects of two intermittent schedules of immediate and nonimmediate punishment. Journal of Psychology, 42, 3–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bacotti, A. V. (1978). Responding under schedules combining response-dependent and response-independent shock delivery. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, 267–272.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (1989). Quantitative prediction and molar description of the environment. The Behavior Analyst, 12, 167–176.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (1991). Glossary. In I. H. Iversen & K. A. Lattal (Eds.), Techniques in the behavioral and neural sciences: Vol. 6. Experimental analysis of behavior (Part 2, pp. G1–G44). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C., & Sagvolden, T. (1980). Preference for free choice over forced choice in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 34, 77–86.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, A. M. (1989). The detrimental effects of extrinsic reinforcement on “intrinsic” motivation. The Behavior Analyst, 12, 1–15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B. (1965). Arbitrary and natural reinforcement. The Psychological Record, 17, 341–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, T. J., & Lattal, K. A. (1990). Stimulus control of behavioral history. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 5–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1969). An outline of psychoanalysis (rev. ed.). New York: Norton. (Original work published 1940)

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon, J., Berryman, R., & Thompson, R. L. (1974). Contingency spaces and measures in classical and instrumental conditioning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 585–605.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, D. A. (1967). Effects of d-amphetamine on multiple schedule performance in the pigeon. Psychonomic Science, 7, 3–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gleeson, S., & Lattal, K. A. (1987). Response-reinforcer relations and the maintenance of behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, 383–393.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Glenn, S. S., Ellis, J., & Greenspoon, J. (1992). On the revolutionary nature of the operant as a unit of behavioral selection. American Psychologist, 47, 1329–1336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, L., & Shimp, C. P. (1975). Reinforcement of behavioral patterns: Sha** a scallop. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 23, 3–16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Heidbreder, E. (1933). Seven psychologies. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J. (1970). On the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, 243–266.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J., & Hineline, P. N. (1966). Negative reinforcement as shock frequency reduction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 421–430.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hineline, P. N. (1992). A self-interpretive behavior analysis. American Psychologist, 47, 1274–1285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iwata, B. A. (1988). The development and adoption of controversial default technologies. The Behavior Analyst, 11, 149–157.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1963). Pragmatism. Cleveland, OH: Meridian Books. (Original work published 1907)

    Google Scholar 

  • Killeen, P. (1978). Superstition: A matter of bias, not detectability. Science, 199, 88–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kohn, A. (1989, January). Incentives can be bad for business. Inc., pp. 93–94.

  • Kornorski, J., & Miller, S. (1937). On two types of conditioned reflex. Journal of General Psychology, 16, 264–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lattal, K. A. (1974). Combinations of response-reinforcer dependence and independence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, 357–362.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lattal, K. A. (1975). Reinforcement contingencies as discriminative stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 23, 241–246.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lattal, K. A. (1979). Reinforcement contingencies as discriminative stimuli: II. Effects of changes in stimulus probability. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 31, 15–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lattal, K. A. (1981). Reinforcement contingencies as discriminative stimuli: Implications for schedule performance. In M. L. Commons & J. A. Nevin (Eds.), Quantitative studies of operant behavior: Discriminative properties of reinforcement schedules (pp. 113–133). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lattal, K. A. (1989). Contingencies on response rate and resistance to change. Learning and Motivation, 20, 191–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lattal, K. A. (1991). Scheduling positive reinforcers. In I. H. Iversen & K. A. Lattal (Eds.), Techniques in the behavioral and neural sciences: Vol. 6. Experimental analysis of behavior (Part 1, pp. 87–134). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lattal, K. A., Freeman, T. J., & Critchfield, T. (1989). Dependency location in interval schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 101–117.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lattal, K. A., & Gleeson, S. (1990). Response acquisition with delayed reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 27–39.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lattal, K. A., & Maxey, G. C. (1971). Some effects of response-independent reinforcers in multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 16, 225–231.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lattal, K. A., & Metzger, B. A. (1994). Response acquisition by Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens) with delayed visual reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 61, 35–44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lattal, K. A., & Poling, A. D. (1982). Describing response-event relations: Babel revisited. The Behavior Analyst, 4, 143–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lattal, K. A., & Ziegler, D. R. (1982). Briefly delayed reinforcement: An interresponse time analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 407–416.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R., & Greene, D. (Eds.). (1978). The hidden cost of reward: New perspectives on the psychology of human motivation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morse, W. H., & Kelleher, R. T. (1977). Determinants of reinforcement and punishment. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 174–188). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nevin, J. A. (1974). Response strength in multiple schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 389–408.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nevin, J. A., Smith, L. D., & Roberts, J. (1987). Does contingent reinforcement strengthen operant behavior? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, 17–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nussear, V. P., & Lattal, K. A. (1983). Discriminative stimulus properties of brief disruptions in response-reinforcer temporal contiguity. Learning and Motivation, 14, 472–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peele, D. B., Casey, J., & Silberberg, A. (1984). Primacy of interresponse-time reinforcement in accounting for rate differences under variable-ratio and variable-interval schedules. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 149–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perone, M., Galizio, M., & Baron, A. (1988). The relevance of animal-based principles in the laboratory study of human operant behavior. In G. Davey & C. Cullen (Eds.), Human operant conditioning and behavior modification (pp. 59–85). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • The Random House dictionary of the English language. (1971). New York: Random House.

  • Redd, W. H. (1969). Effects of mixed reinforcement contingencies on adults control of children’s behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 2, 249–254.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Redd, W. H., & Birnbrauer, J. S. (1969). Adults as discriminative stimuli for different reinforcement contingencies with retarded children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 7, 440–447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A. (1967). Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. Psychological Review, 74, 71–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, G. K., & Platt, J. R. (1985). Models of response-reinforcer contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 152–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shanks, D. R., Pearson, S. M., & Dickinson, A. (1989). Temporal contiguity and the judgement of causality by human subjects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41B, 139–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent verbal classes. In T. Thompson & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units (pp. 213–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sizemore, O. J., & Lattal, K. A. (1977). Dependency, temporal contiguity, and response-independent reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 119–125.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sizemore, O. J., & Lattal, K. A. (1978). Unsignaled delay of reinforcement in variable-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 30, 169–175.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1948). “Superstition” in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 168–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B.F. (1961a). The generic nature of the concepts of stimulus and response. In Cumulative record (enlarged ed., pp. 347–366). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. (Reprinted from The Journal of General Psychology, 1935, 12, 40–65)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1961b). Two types of conditioned reflex: A reply to Kornorski and Miller. In Cumulative record (enlarged ed., pp. 376–383). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. (Reprinted from The Journal of General Psychology, 1937, 16, 272–279)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staddon, J. E. R. (1992). The “superstition” experiment: A reversible figure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 270–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staddon, J. E. R., & Simmelhag, V. L. (1971). The “superstition” experiment: A reexamination of its implications for the principles of adaptive behavior. Psychological Review, 78, 3–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (1973). Classical conditioning. In J. A. Nevin & G. S. Reynolds (Eds.), The study of behavior: Learning, motivation, emotion, and instinct (pp. 70–112). Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal intelligence. New York: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wanchisen, B. A., Tathum, T. A., & Mooney, S. E. (1989). Variable-ratio conditioning history produces high- and low-rate fixed-interval performance in rats. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 167–179.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer, M. (1972). Fundamental issues in psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiener, P. P. (Ed.). (1958). Charles S. Peirce: Selected writings. New York: Dover.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilcoxon, H. A. (1969). Historical introduction to the problem of reinforcement. In J. T. Tapp (Ed.), Reinforcement and behavior (pp. 1–46). New York: Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, B. A. (1976). The effects of unsignaled delayed reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 26, 441–449.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, B. A. (1983). Revising the principle of reinforcement. Behaviorism, 11, 63–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeiler, M. D. (1968). Fixed and variable schedules of response independent reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 405–414.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zeiler, M. D. (1972). Superstitious behavior in children: An experimental analysis. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 7, pp. 1–29). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuriff, G. E. (1986). Behaviorism: A conceptual reconstruction. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kennon A. Lattal.

Additional information

This article is based on the 1994 Presidential Address at the 20th annual convention of the Association for Behavior Analysis, held in Atlanta, Georgia.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lattal, K.A. Contingency and Behavior Analysis. BEHAV ANALYST 18, 209–224 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392709

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392709

Key words

Navigation