This entry has three sections. The first concerns the beginnings of evolutionary psychology in the late nineteenth century. The second briefly provides a historical background, set between 1900 and 1975, for the third section, which concerns evolutionary psychology from about 1975 to mid-2010.

Late Nineteenth-Century Evolutionary Psychology

Anaximander, a pre-Socratic philosopher of the sixth century BCE, was quoted by the Roman scholar Plutarch as having said that

[At] the beginning man was generated from all sorts of animals, since all the rest can quickly get food for themselves, but man alone requires careful feeding for a long time; such a being at the beginning could not have preserved his existence. (Nahm 1964, pp. 42–43)

Moreover, Plato wrote that Anaximander declared

[Not] that fish and men were generated at the same time, but that at first men were generated in the form of fishes, and that growing up as sharks do till they were able to help themselves, they then came forth on...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
EUR 29.95
Price includes VAT (France)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
EUR 533.93
Price includes VAT (France)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
EUR 632.99
Price includes VAT (France)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free ship** worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Alcock, J., & Crawford, C. (2008). Evolutionary questions for evolutionary psychologists. In C. Crawford & D. Krebs (Eds.), Foundations of evolutionary psychology (pp. 25–45). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum/Taylor & Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, R. D., Hoogland, J. D., Howard, R. D., Noonan, K. M., & Sherman, P. W. (1979). Sexual dimorphism and breeding systems in pinnipeds, ungulates, primates, and humans. In N. A. Chagan & W. Irons (Eds.), Evolutionary psychology and social behavior: An anthropological perspective (pp. 402–435). North Scitnate: Duxbury Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1990). The adaptive character of thought. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R., Bothell, D., Byrne, M. D., Douglass, S., Lebiere, C., & Qin, Y. (2004). An integrated theory of the mind. Psychological Review, 111, 1036–1060.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R., & Gallup, G. G. (1997). Self-recognition in Sanguinus? A critical essay. Animal Behaviour, 54, 1563–1567.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R., & Milson, R. (1989). Human memory: an adaptive perspective. Psychological Review, 96, 703–719.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. A. (2005). From monkeylike action recognition to human language: an evolutionary framework for linguistics. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 105–167.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aristotle (1910). Historia animalium. (trans: Thompson, D. W.). In J. A. Smith, & W. D. Ross (Eds.), The works of Aristotle (Vol. IV). Oxford: Clarendon Press (Original work written ca. 362 BCE).

    Google Scholar 

  • Aunger, R. (2007). Memes. In R. I. M. Dunbar & L. Barrett (Eds.), Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 599–604). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 211, 1390–1396.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Badcock, C. (2008). An evolutionary theory of mind and mental illness: Genetic conflict and the mentalistic continuum. In C. Crawford & D. Krebs (Eds.), Foundations of evolutionary psychology (pp. 433–452). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum/Taylor & Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balaban, E. (2006). Cognitive developmental biology: history, process and fortune’s wheel. Cognition, 101, 298–332.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1895). Mental development in the child and the race. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1896). A new factor in evolution. American Naturalist, 30, 441–451, 536–554.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1902). Development and evolution. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barash, Y., Calarco, J. A., Gao, W., Pan, Q., Wang, X., Shai, O., et al. (2010). Deciphering the splicing code. Nature, 465, 53–59.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Boston: MIT Press/Bradford Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2005a). The empathizing system: A revision of the 1994 model of the Mindreading System. In B. Ellis & D. F. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 468–492). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2005b). Prenatal testosterone in mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2008). The evolution of brain mechanisms for social behavior. In C. Crawford & D. Krebs (Eds.), Foundations of evolutionary psychology (pp. 415–432). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum/Taylor & Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, H. C., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2007). The hominid entry into the cognitive niche. In S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 241–248). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barton, R. A., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (1997). The evolution of the social brain. In A. Whiten & R. W. Byrne (Eds.), Machiavellian intelligence II: Extensions and evaluations (pp. 240–263). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton, D. (1995). Language and human behavior. London: University College London Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boakes, R. (1984). From Darwin to behaviorism. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burton, M., & Burton, R. (Eds.) (1974). Cheetah. In Funk & Wagnalls wildlife encyclopedia (Vol. 4, pp. 404–406). New York: Funk & Wagnalls (Original work published 1969).

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (2003). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating (Rev. edn). New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (2007). The evolution of human strategies: Consequences for conflict and cooperation. In S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 375–382). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (2009). The great struggles of life: Darwin and the emergence of evolutionary psychology. The American Psychologist, 64, 140–148.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W. (1996). Machiavellian intelligence. Evolutionary Anthropology, 5, 172–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W. (1997). The technical intelligence hypothesis: An additional evolutionary stimulus to intelligence? In A. Whiten & R. W. Byrne (Eds.), Machiavellian intelligence II: Extensions and evaluations (pp. 289–311). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W., & Corp, N. (2004). Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, 271, 1693–1699.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (Eds.). (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cahan, E. D. (1984). The genetic psychologies of James Mark Baldwin and Jean Piaget. Developmental Psychology, 20, 128–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell, M. C., & Caldwell, D. K. (1966). Epimeletic (care-giving) behavior in cetaceans. In K. S. Norris (Ed.), Whales, dolphins and porpoises (pp. 755–789). Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carr, L. M., Iacoboni, M. C., Dubeau, M. C., Mazziotta, J. C., & Lenzi, G. L. (2003). Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 5497–5502.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chabris, C., & Simon, D. (2010). The invisible gorilla and other ways our intuitions deceive us. New York: Crown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of Verbal behavior by B.F. Skinner. Language, 35, 25–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cock, A. G., & Forsdyke, D. R. (2008). Treasure your exceptions: The science and life of William Bateson. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (1991). The lopsided ape: Evolution of the generative mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (2003). From mouth to hand: gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 199–260.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (2004). FOXP2 and the mirror system. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 95–96.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1987). From evolution to behavior: Evolutionary psychology as the missing link. In J. Dupre (Ed.), The latest on the best: Essays on evolution on optimality (pp. 277–306). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1989). Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, Part II. Case study: a computational theory of social exchange. Ethology and Sociobiology, 10, 51–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992a). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–36). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992b). Cognitive adaptations for social change. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 163–227). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1994). Better than rational: evolutionary psychology and the invisible hand. The American Economic Review, 84, 327–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–185.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A., & Clifton, C., Jr. (1999). Comprehending spoken language: A blueprint of the listener. In C. M. Brown & P. Hagoort (Eds.), The neurocognition of language (pp. 123–166). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1899). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex (2nd ed.). London: John Murray (Original work published 1871).

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1904). The expression of emotions in man and animals. (2nd edn, F. Darwin, Ed.). London: John Murray (Original work published 1872).

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (2004). On the origin of species by means of natural selection or, the preservation of favored races in the struggle for life. London: Collector’s Library, CRW (Original work published 1859).

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, T. (1943). Chambers’s twentieth century dictionary. (Revised and expanded by J. L. Geddie). Edinburgh: W. & R. Chambers (Original work published 1901).

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (2006). The selfish gene (30th Anniversary Edition). Oxford: Oxford University (Original work published 1976).

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (1996). Good natured: The origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (2004). On the possibility of animal empathy. In T. Manstead, N. Frijda, & A. Fischer (Eds.), Feelings and emotions: The Amsterdam symposium (pp. 379–399). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (2005). Homo homni lupus? Morality, the social instincts and our fellow primates. In J.-P. Changeux, A. R. Damasio, W. Singer, & Y. Christen (Eds.), Neurobiology of human values (pp. 17–35). Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex among apes. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press (Original work published 1982).

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 279–300.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (2009). The age of empathy: Nature’s lessons for a kinder society. Toronto: McClelland & Stewart.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (2010). Morality and its relation to primate social instincts. In H. Høgh-Olesen (Ed.), Human morality and sociality: Evolutionary and comparative perspectives (pp. 31–57). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M., & Aureli, F. (1996). Consolation, reconciliation, and a possible cognitive difference between macaque and chimpanzee. In A. E. Russon, K. A. Bard, & S. T. Parker (Eds.), Reaching into thought: The minds of the great apes (pp. 80–110). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M., & van Roosmalen, A. (1979). Reconciliation and consolation among chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 5, 55–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delboeuf, J. (2010). A law of mathematics applicable to the theory of [biological] transformation (trans: Forsdyke, P. V., & Forsdyke, D. R.). http://post.queens.ca/~forsdyke/gulick.htm (Original work published 1877).

  • Dewsbury, D. A. (2009). Charles Darwin and psychology at the bicentennial and sesquicentennial: an introduction. The American Psychologist, 64, 67–74.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (2001). A mind so rare: The evolution of human consciousness. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (1988). Primate social systems. London: Croom Helm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6(5), 178–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). Why are apes so smart? In P. Kappeler & M. Pereira (Eds.), Primate life histories and socioecology (pp. 285–298). Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007). Evolution of the social brain. In S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 280–286). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. (1987). Neural Darwinism. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. (1972). Emotion in the human face: Guidelines for research and an integration of findings. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ermer, E., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2007). Functional specialization and the adaptionist program. In S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 153–160). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, R. A. (1930). The genetical theory of natural selection. Oxford, UK: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, S. E. (2006). Tangled webs: tracing the connections between genes and cognition. Cognition, 101, 270–297.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flack, J. C., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2000). Any animal whatever: Darwinian building blocks of morality in monkeys and apes. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7, 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foley, R. (1995). Humans before humanity. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forsdyke, D. R. (2001). The Origin of Species revisited: A Victorian who anticipated modern developments in Darwin’s theory. Montreal: McGill-Queen’s University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forsdyke, D. R. (2006). Evolutionary bioinformatics. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fossey, D. (1984). Gorillas in the mist. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. (2000). The psychologist’s view. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotion (2nd ed., pp. 59–74). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galdikas, B. M. F. (1995). Reflections of Eden: My years with the orangutans in Borneo. Toronto: Little Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galton, F. (1888). Co-relations and their measurements, chiefly from anthropometric data. Proceedings of the Royal Society, 45, 135–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver-Apgar, C. E. (2005). Adaptations to ovulation. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 344–371). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gat, A. (2010). The causes of war in natural and historical evolution. In H. Høgh-Olesen (Ed.), Human morality and sociality: Evolutionary and comparative perspectives (pp. 160–190). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. (2008). Human: The science behind what makes us unique. New York: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, G. C. (2000). Evolution and proximate expression of human paternal investment. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 55–77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, G. C. (2005). Evolution of paternal investment. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 483–488). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghiselin, M. T. (1973). Darwin and evolutionary psychology. Science, 179, 964–968.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. (1981). Messung und Modellbildung in der Psychologie [Measurement and model-building in psychology]. Munich: Ernst Reinhardt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Hug, K. (1992). Domain-specific reasoning: social contracts, cheating, and perspective change. Cognition, 43, 127–171.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Murray, D. J. (1987). Cognition as intuitive statistics. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & ABC Research Group. (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smarter. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, G. (1979). Comparative psychology and ethology. In E. Hearst (Ed.), The first century of experimental psychology (pp. 147–173). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, 205, 581–591.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J., & Vrba, E. (1982). Exaptation – a missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology, 8, 4–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, C. D. (2009). Darwinian theory, functionalism, and the first American psychological revolution. The American Psychologist, 64, 75–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grinder, R. E. (1967). A history of genetic psychology: The first science of human development. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grodzinsky, Y. (2000). The neurology of syntax: language without Broca’s areas. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 1–71.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, W. D. (1964a). The genetical evolution of social behaviour I. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–16.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, W. D. (1964b). The genetical evolution of social behaviour II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 17–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hardy, A. C. (1960). Was man aquatic in the past? New Scientist, 7, 642–645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harman, O. (2010). The price of altruism: George Price and the search for the origins of kindness. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Kralik, J., Borro-Mahan, C., Garret, M., & Oser, J. (1995). Self-recognition in primates: phylogeny and the salience of species-typical features. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 92, 10811–10814.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Miller, C. T., Liu, K., & Gupta, R. (2001). Cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) fail to show mirror-guided self-exploration. American Journal of Primatology, 53, 131–137.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, U., & Thibault, P. (2009). Darwin and emotion expression. The American Psychologist, 64, 120–128.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffer, J. (2007). Comparative literature. New York Times Book Review, Apr 15, p. 27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Homans, G. C. (1961). Social behavior: Its elementary forms. New York: Harcourt Brace & World.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howe, M. L., & Courage, M. L. (1993). On resolving the enigma of infantile amnesia. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 305–326.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M. (2009). Mirroring people: The science of empathy and how we connect with others. New York: Picador (Original work published 2008).

    Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder, B. (1998). Genetic epistemology and developmental psychology. In R. W. Rieber & K. Salzinger (Eds.), Psychology: Theoretical-historical perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 411–421). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jablonski, J. H., & Chaplin, G. (2000). The evolution of human skin coloration. Journal of Human Evolution, 39, 57–106.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jaynes, J. (1977). The origin of consciousness in the breakdown of the bicameral mind. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, C. Y. (2010). Author on leave after Harvard inquiry: Investigations of scientist’s work finds evidence of misconduct, prompts retraction by journal. The Boston Globe. http://www.boston.com.

  • Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (Eds.). (1982). Judgments under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kandel, E. R. (2006). In search of memory: The emergence of a new science of mind. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kareev, Y. (2000). Seven (indeed, plus or minus two) and the detection of correlations. Psychological Review, 107, 397–402.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, S. (2007). The evolution of language. In R. I. M. Dunbar & L. Barrett (Eds.), Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 669–681). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knobloch, C. (1988). Geschichte der psychologischen Sprachauffassungen in Deutschland von 1850 bis 1920 [A history of ideas about the psychology of language in Germany from 1850 to 1920]. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koffka, K. (1925). The growth of the mind (2nd edn) (trans: Ogden, F. M.). New York: Harcourt Brace (Original work published 1921).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kudo, H., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2001). Neocortex size and social network size in primates. Animal Behavior, 62, 711–722.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lai, C. S. L., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Vargha-Kadem, F., & Monaco, A. P. (2001). A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in severe speech and language disorder. Nature, 413, 519–523.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lesch, J. E. (1976). Romanes, George John. In C. C. Gillespie (Ed.), Dictionary of scientific biography (Vol. 11, pp. 516–520). New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1999). Producing spoken language of the speaker. In C. M. Brown & P. Hagoort (Eds.), The neurocognition of language (pp. 83–122). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–75.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M. B. (1995). Cognition-emotion feedback and the self-organization of developmental paths. Human Development, 38, 71–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewontin, R. C. (1961). Evolution and the theory of games. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 1, 382–403.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P. (1984). The biology and evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P. (1991). Uniquely human: The evolution of speech, thought, and selfless behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacNeilage, P. F., & Davis, B. L. (2005). The evolution of language. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 698–723). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malamuth, N. M., Huppin, M., & Paul, B. (2005). Sexual coercion. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 394–418). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. (2008). Kluge: The haphazard construction of the human mind. New York: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1966). Human sexual response. Boston: Little Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J. (1964). Group selection and kin selection. Nature, 201, 1145–1147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J. (1972). Game theory and the evolution of fighting. In J. Maynard Smith (Ed.), On evolution (pp. 8–28). Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J. (1977). Parental investment: a prospective analysis. Animal Behaviour, 25, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J., & Price, G. R. (1973). The logic of animal conflict. Nature, 246, 15–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCosh, J. (1875). The Scottish philosophy, biographical, expository, critical from Hutcheson to Hamilton. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDougall, W. (1931). An introduction to social psychology (22nd edn, enlarged). London: Methuen & Co. Ltd. (Original work published in 1908).

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 74–78.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mercer, T., & McKeown, D. (2010). Interference in short-term auditory memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1256–1265.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. (2007). Brain evolution. In S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 287–293). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mills, W. (1898). The nature and development of animal intelligence. London: T. Fisher Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mills, W. (1905). The nature of animal intelligence and the methods of investigating it. Psychological Review, 6, 262–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Møller, A. P. (2008). Sex and sexual selection. In C. Crawford & D. Krebs (Eds.), Foundations of evolutionary psychology (pp. 71–90). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum/Taylor & Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, T. H. (1926). The theory of the gene. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, D. (1967). The naked ape: A zoologist’s study of the human animal. New York: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, F. M. (1866). Lectures on the science of language. London: Longmans, Green.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, G. (1949). Historical introduction to modern psychology (Rev. edn). New York: Harcourt, Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, D. J. (1988). A history of Western psychology (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, D. J. (1990a). On the early history of psycholinguistics. Historiographia Linguistica, 17, 369–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, D. J. (1990b). A Canadian pioneer of comparative psychology: T. Wesley Mills (1947–1915). International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 3, 205–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, D. J., & Farahmand, B. (1998). Gestalt psychology and evolutionary psychology. In R. W. Rieber & K. Salzinger (Eds.), Psychology: Theoretical-historical perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 254–287). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nahm, M. C. (1964). Selections from early Greek philosophy. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesse, R. M. (2004). Natural selection and the elusiveness of happiness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B, 359, 1333–1347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesse, R. M., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2009). Evolution, emotions, and emotional disorders. The American Psychologist, 64, 129–139.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford, UK: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Regan, J. K., & Noë, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 939–1031.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rhinehart, and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (2007). Mind and its evolution: A dual coding theoretical approach. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, M. B., Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2010). Evolutionary psychology and criminal justice: A recalibrational theory of punishment and reconciliation. In H. Høgh-Olesen (Ed.), Human morality and sociality: Evolutionary and comparative perspectives (pp. 72–131). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plotkin, H. (2004). Evolutionary thought in psychology: A brief history. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porter, N. (1877). The elements of intellectual science. New York: Scribner, Armstrong.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–72.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Preston, G. E. W., & Trew, V. C. G. (1928). Studies in evolutionary psychology; illustrated from the history of science. London: Theosophical.

    Google Scholar 

  • Provine, R. R., Krosnowski, K. A., & Brocato, N. W. (2009). Tearing: Breakthrough in human emotional signaling. Evolutionary Psychology, 7, 52–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reilly, R. G. (2002). The relationship between object manipulation and language development in Broca’s area: a connectionist account of Greenfield’s hypothesis. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 145–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiss, D., & Marino, L. (2001). Mirror self-recognition in the bottle-nose dolphin: a case of cognitive convergence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 98, 5937–5942.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridley, M. (2010). The rational optimist: How prosperity evolves. New York: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2006). Mirrors in the mind. Scientific American, 295(5), 54–61.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, G. J. (1882). Animal intelligence. London: Kegan Paul, Trench.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, G. J. (1883). Mental evolution in animals. London: Kegan Paul, Trench.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, G. J. (1886). Physiological selection: an additional suggestion on the origin of species. Nature, 34, 314–316, 336–340, 362–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, G. J. (1888). Mental evolution in man. London: Kegan Paul, Trench.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, G. J. (1897). Darwin, and after Darwin: Post-Darwinian questions, isolation and physiological selection (C. Lloyd Morgan, Ed.). Chicago: Open Court.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salmon, C., & Crawford, C. (2008). Evolutionary psychology: The historical context. In C. Crawford & D. Krebs (Eds.), Foundations of evolutionary psychology (pp. 1–21). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum/Taylor & Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 293–317). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheyd, G. J., Garver-Apgar, C. E., & Gangestad, S. W. (2008). Physical attractiveness: Signals of phenotypic quality and beyond. In C. Crawford & D. Krebs (Eds.), Foundations of evolutionary psychology (pp. 239–259). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum/Taylor & Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. P. (2005). Fundamentals of mating strategies. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 258–291). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schröger, E. (2007). Mismatch negativity: a microphone into auditory memory. Journal of Psychophysiology, 21, 138–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, J. S. (2010). Darwin’s disciple: George John Romanes, a life in letters. Philadelphia: American Philosophical Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shorter, E. (2005). Written in the flesh: A history of desire. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’ Doherty, J. P., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory component of pain. Science, 303, 1157–1162.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Empathic neural responses are modulated by the fairness of others. Nature, 439, 466–469.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. D., Shields, W. E., & Washburn, D. A. (2003). The comparative psychology of uncertainty monitoring and metacognition. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 317–373.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, H. (1890). The principles of psychology (3rd ed.). London: Williams & Norgate (Original work published 1855).

    Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf, T., & Whiten, A. (2001). Mental evolution and development: evidence for secondary representation in children, great apes, and other animals. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 629–650.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sugiyama, L. S. (2005). Physical attractiveness in adaptationist perspective. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 292–343). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Symons, D. (1979). The evolution of human sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thornhill, R., & Thornhill, N. W. (1992). The evolutionary psychology of men’s coercive sexuality. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 363–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd, P. M., & Gigerenzer, G. (2007). Mechanisms of ecological rationality: Heuristics and environments that make us smart. In R. I. M. Dunbar & L. Barrett (Eds.), Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 197–210). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd, P. M., Hertwig, R., & Hoffrage, U. (2005). Evolutionary cognitive psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 776–802). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd, P. M., & Miller, G. F. (1999). From pride and prejudice to persuasion: Satisficing in mate search. In G. Gigerenzer, P. M. Todd, & ABC Research Group (Eds.), Simple heuristics that make us smart (pp. 287–308). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and man. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2010). Groups in mind: The coalitional roots of war and morality. In H. Høgh-Olesen (Ed.), Human morality and sociality: Evolutionary and comparative perspectives (pp. 194–234). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R. L., & Hare, H. (1976). Haplodiploidy and the evolution of the social insects. Science, 191, 249–263.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A. (1972). Elimination by aspects: a theory of choice. Psychological Review, 79, 281–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waddington, C. M. (1957). The strategy of the genes: A discussion of some aspects of theoretical biology. London: George Allen and Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1930). Behaviorism (Rev. edn). New York: W. W. Norton (Original work published 1924).

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. D. (2003). DNA: The secret of life. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. D., & Crick, F. H. C. (1953). Molecular structure of nucleic acids: a structure for deoxyribose nucleic acid. Nature, 171, 737–738.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, B. H., & Depew, D. J. (Eds.). (2003). Evolution and learning: The Baldwin effect reconsidered. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weldon, W. F. R. (1902). Mendel’s laws of alternative inheritance in peas. Biometrika, 1, 228–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Westermarck, E. (1894). The history of human marriage (2nd ed.). London: Macmillan (Original work published 1891).

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection: A critique of some current evolutionary thought. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, G. C. (1971). Group selection. Chicago: Aldine-Atherton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, G. C. (1975). Sex and evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. O. (1971). The insect societies. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. O. (2000). Sociobiology: The new synthesis (20th Anniversary Edition). Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press (Original work published 1975)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wozniak, R. H. (1998). Thought and things: James Mark Baldwin and the biosocial origins of mind. In R. W. Rieber & K. Salzinger (Eds.), Psychology: Theoretical-historical perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 429–453). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, S. (1922). Coefficients of inbreeding and relationship. The American Naturalist, 56, 330–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, S. (1931). Evolution of Mendelian populations. Genetics, 16, 97–158.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1973). The language of gestures (trans: Thayer, J. S., Greenleaf, C. M., & Silberman, M. D.). The Hague: Mouton (Original work published 1921).

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, T. (2002). Archaeology and cognitive evolution. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 389–438.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wynne-Edwards, V. C. (1962). Animal dispersion in relation to social behavior. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection – a selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205–214.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. (1995). Altruism as a handicap: the limitations of kin selection and reciprocity. Journal of Avian Biology, 26, 1–3.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David J. Murray .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this entry

Cite this entry

Barnes, M.E., Murray, D.J. (2012). Evolutionary Psychology. In: Rieber, R.W. (eds) Encyclopedia of the History of Psychological Theories. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-0463-8_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-0463-8_4

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4419-0425-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4419-0463-8

  • eBook Packages: Behavioral Science

Publish with us

Policies and ethics

Navigation