Log in

Disgust, Sexual Cues, and the Prophylaxis Hypothesis

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Evolutionary Psychological Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Women’s susceptibility to infection has been found to vary across the menstrual cycle. During the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle, when progesterone levels are at their peak, women experience a downregulation in inflammatory immune responses to tolerate the presence of foreign paternal genetic material. The prophylaxis hypothesis holds that, during such periods of physiological immune vulnerability, women will engage in increased prophylactic behavior in response to cues associated with pathogen transmission (like sexual cues) to guard against infection. The current study examined disgust and other proposed prophylactic responses (i.e., attention and desire for solitary and dyadic sexual activity) in response to sexual and nonsexual films among naturally cycling women (N = 21) during the follicular and luteal phases of their menstrual cycles. No significant differences were found during the follicular and luteal phase on disgust, attention, or desire for solitary or dyadic sexual activity. Strong negative associations were found between feelings of disgust to sexual stimuli and proposed prophylactic behaviors (attention, desire for sexual activity with a partner) that were most prominent during the luteal phase of women’s menstrual cycles, suggesting that they may have served as a prophylactic mechanism, protecting women’s bodies from infection during a period of immune vulnerability. However, contrary to hypothesis, no significant associations were found between progesterone (the hormone that regulates changes in immune functioning) and proposed prophylactic responses. Further research examining prophylactic effects in response to sexual stimuli is warranted.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Desire for dyadic sexual activity and solitary sexual activity were analyzed as distinct constructs in lieu of a contrast score for ease of interpretation.

  2. A similar pattern emerged when z- and log-transformed values were used instead of raw progesterone data.

References

  • Alexander, N. J. (1990). Sexual transmission of human immunodeficiency virus: virus entry into the male and female genital tract. Fertility and Sterility, 54(1), 1–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews III, A. R., Crone, T., Cholka, C. B., Cooper, T. V., & Bridges, A. J. (2015). Correlational and experimental analyses of the relation between disgust and sexual arousal. Motivation and Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-015-9485-y.

  • Angyal, A. (1941). Disgust and related aversions. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 36(3), 393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bakos, O., Lundkvist, L., Wide, O., & Bergh, T. (1994). Ultrasonographical and hormonal description of the normal ovulatory menstrual cycle. Acta Obstetrica et Gynecologica Scandanavica, 73, 790–796.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bancroft, J., Graham, C. A., Janssen, E., & Sanders, S. A. (2009). The dual control model: current status and future directions. Journal of Sex Research, 46(2–3), 121–142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borg, C., & de Jong, P. J. (2012). Feelings of disgust and disgust-induced avoidance weaken following induced sexual arousal in women. PLoS One, 7(9), e44111.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Borg, C., de Jong, P. J., & Georgiadis, J. R. (2014). Subcortical BOLD responses during visual sexual stimulation vary as a function of implicit porn associations in women. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(2), 158–166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bossio, J. A., Suschinsky, K. D., Puts, D. A., & Chivers, M. L. (2014). Does menstrual cycle phase influence the gender specificity of heterosexual women’s genital and subjective sexual arousal? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43(5), 941–952.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Both, S., Everaerd, W., & Laan, E. (2007). Desire emerges from excitement. A psychophysiological perspective on sexual motivation. In E. Janssen (Ed.), The psychophysiology of sex (pp. 327–339). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chakraborty, H., Sen, P. K., Helms, R. W., Vernazza, P. L., Fiscus, S. A., Eron, J. J., et al. (2001). Viral burden in genital secretions determines male-to-female sexual transmission of HIV-1: a probabilistic empiric model. AIDS, 15(5), 621–627.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chiazze, L., Brayer, F. T., MacIsco, J. J., Parker, M. P., & Duffy, B. J. (1968). The length and variability of the human menstrual cycle. Journal of the American Medical Association, 203, 377–380.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chivers, M. L., Rieger, G., Latty, E., & Bailey, J. M. (2004). A sex difference in the specificity of sexual arousal. Psychological Science, 15, 736–744.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chivers, M. L., Seto, M. C., & Blanchard, R. (2007). Gender and sexual orientation differences in sexual response to sexual activities versus gender of actors in sexual films. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 1108–1121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, W. R. (2007). In defense of self: how the immune system really works in managing health and disease. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway, C. A., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Welling, L. L. M., Smith, M. L., Perrett, D. I., et al. (2007). Salience of emotional displays of danger and contagion in faces is enhanced when progesterone levels are raised. Hormones and Behaviour, 51(2), 202–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corner, G. W., & Allen, W. M. (1929). Physiology of the corpus luteum. II. Production of a special uterine reaction (progestational proliferation) by extracts of the corpus luteum. American Journal of Physiology, 88(2), 326–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Critchley, H. O., Kelly, R. W., Brenner, R. M., & Baird, D. T. (2001). The endocrinology of menstruation—a role for the immune system. Clinical Endocrinology, 55(6), 701–710.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, V., Aunger, R., & Rabie, T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society Biological Sciences, 271, 131–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of emotions in man and animal. Chicago: University of Chicago Press (Original work published 1872).

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, S. J., & Chivers, M. L. (2014). Gender-specificity of solitary and dyadic sexual desire among gynephilic and androphilic women and men. The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 11(4), 980–994.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P. J., & Peters, M. L. (2007). Contamination vs. harm-relevant outcome expectancies and covariation bias in spider phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 1271–1284.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P. J., van Overveld, M., & Borg, C. (2013). Giving in to arousal or staying stuck in disgust? Disgust-based mechanisms in sex and sexual dysfunction. Journal of Sex Research, 50(3–4), 247–262.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dillon, K. M., & Brooks, D. (1992). Unusual cleaning behaviour in the luteal phase. Psychological Reports, 70(1), 35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17(2), 124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1986). A new pan-cultural facial expression of emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 10(2), 159–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221(4616), 1208–1210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellison, P. T. (1988). Human salivary steroids: methodological considerations and applications in physical anthropology. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 31(S9), 115–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fessler, D. M. (2001). Luteal phase immunosuppression and meat eating. Rivista di Biologia Biology Forum, 94(3), 403–426.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fessler, D. T. (2002). Reproductive immunosuppression and diet. Current Anthropology, 43(1), 19–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fessler, D. M., & Navarrete, C. D. (2003). Domain-specific variation in disgust sensitivity across the menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(6), 406–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fessler, D. M. T., Pillsworth, E. G., & Flamson, T. J. (2004). Angry men and disgusted women: an evolutionary approach to the influence of emotions on risk taking. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 95, 107–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fessler, D. M., Eng, S. J., & Navarrete, C. D. (2005). Elevated disgust sensitivity in the first trimester of pregnancy: evidence supporting the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(4), 344–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flaxman, S. M., & Sherman, P. W. (2000). Morning sickness: a mechanism for protecting mother and embryo. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 75, 113–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fleischman, D. S. (2014). Women’s disgust adaptations. In Evolutionary perspectives on human sexual psychology and behavior (pp. 277–296). New York: Springer.

  • Fleischman, D. S., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2011). Progesterone’s effects on the psychology of disease avoidance: support for the compensatory behavioural prophylaxis hypothesis. Hormones and Behaviour, 59(2), 271–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleischman, D. S., Hamilton, L. D., Fessler, D. M., & Meston, C. M. (2015). Disgust versus lust: exploring the interactions of disgust and fear with sexual arousal in women. PLoS One, 10(6), e0118151.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fumagalli, M., Sironi, M., Pozzoli, U., Ferrer-Admetlla, A., Pattini, L., & Nielsen, R. (2011). Signatures of environmental genetic adaptation pinpoint pathogens as the main selective pressure throughout human evolution. PLoS Genetics, 7, e1002355.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gangestad, S. W. (2007). Reproductive strategies and tactics. In R. I. M. Dunbar & L. Barrett (Eds.), Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 321–332). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gangestad, S. W., & Haselton, M. G. (2015). Human estrus: implications for relationship science. Current Opinion in Psychology, 1, 45–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geer, J. H., & Bellard, H. S. (1996). Sexual content induced delays in unprimed lexical decisions: gender and context effects. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 25, 379–395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gildersleeve, K., Haselton, M. G., & Fales, M. R. (2014). Do women’s mate preferences change across the ovulatory cycle? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 140(5), 1205–1259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: a scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 701–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J. S., DeLamater, J. D., & Byers, E. S. (2004). Understanding human sexuality: third Canadian edition. Toronto: McGraw-Hill Ryerson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janssen, E., Everaerd, W., Spiering, M., & Janssen, J. (2000). Automatic processes and the appraisal of sexual stimuli: toward an information processing model of sexual arousal. Journal of Sex Research, 37, 8–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L., DeBruine, L. M., Feinberg, D. R., Law Smith, M. J., et al. (2005). Commitment to relationships and preferences for femininity and apparent health in faces are strongest on days of the menstrual cycle when progesterone level is high. Hormones and Behaviour, 48, 283–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, B. C., Hahn, A. C., Fisher, C. I., Wang, H., Kandrik, M., Han, C., … DeBruine, L. M. (2017). Women’s preferences for facial masculinity are not related to their hormonal status. bioRxiv. Preprint retrieved from http://biorxiv.org/content/early/2017/05/11/136549.

  • Karama, S., Armony, J., & Beauregard, M. (2011). Film excerpts shown to specifically elicit various affects lead to overlap** activation foci in a large set of symmetrical brain regions in males. PLoS One, 6(7), e22343.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, D. R. (2011). Yuck!: the nature and moral significance of disgust. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & Gebhard, P. H. (1953). Sexual behavior in the human female. Philadelphia: Saunders.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koukounas, E., & McCabe, M. P. (1997). Sexual and emotional variables influencing sexual response to erotica. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(3), 221–231.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laskin, D. L., Sunil, V. R., Gardner, C. R., & Laskin, J. D. (2011). Macrophages and tissue injury: agents of defense or destruction? Annual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology, 51, 267–288.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, E. M., Ambler, J. K., & Sagarin, B. J. (2014). Effects of subjective sexual arousal on sexual, pathogen, and moral disgust sensitivity in women and men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43, 1115–1121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Y., Zhang, D., & Luo, Y. (2014). How disgust facilitates avoidance: an ERP study on attention modulation by threats. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(4), 598–604.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lochmiller, R. L., & Deerenberg, C. (2000). Trade-offs in evolutionary immunology: just what is the cost of immunity? Oikos, 88, 87–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madkan, V. K., Giancola, A. A., Sra, K. K., & Tyring, S. K. (2006). Sex differences in the transmission, prevention, and disease manifestations of sexually transmitted diseases. Archives of Dermatology, 142(3), 365–370.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meston, C. M., & Frohlich, P. F. (2000). The neurobiology of sexual function. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 1012–1030.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morokoff, P. J. (1986). Volunteer bias in the psychophysiological study of female sexuality. Journal of Sex Research, 22(1), 35–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nallanathan, B., Mendoza, K., Curran, M.J., Lindau, S.T. (2007). Salivary progesterone measurement in wave I of the National Social Life, Health & Aging Project (NSHAP). Chicago Core on Biomarkers in Population-Based Aging Research (http://biomarkers.uchicago.edu/pdfs/TR-Progesterone.pdf).

  • Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. L. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 303–321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olatunji, B. O., Huijding, J., de Jong, P. G., & Smits, J. A. J. (2011). The relative contributions of fear and disgust reductions to improvements in spider phobia following exposure-based treatment. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42, 117–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Panter-Brick, C., Lotstein, D. S., & Ellison, P. T. (1993). Seasonality of reproductive function and weight loss in rural Nepali women. Human Reproduction, 8(5), 684–690.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plutchik, R. (1962). The emotions: facts, theories, and a new model. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puts, D. A. (2006). Cyclic variation in women’s preferences for masculine traits. Human Nature, 17, 114–127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Quigley, J. F., Sherman, M. F., & Sherman, N. C. (1997). Personality disorder symptoms, gender, and age as predictors of adolescent disgust sensitivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 22(5), 661–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Regan, P. C. (1996). Rhythms of desire: the association between menstrual cycle phases and female sexual desire. Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, 5, 145–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohrmann, S., & Hopp, H. (2008). Cardiovascular indicators of disgust. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 68(3), 201–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94(1), 23–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., Nemeroff, C., Horowitz, M., Gordon, B., & Voet, W. (1995). The borders of the self: contamination sensitivity and potency of the body apertures and other body parts. Journal of Research in Personality, 29, 318–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2000). Disgust. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions. New York: Guilford Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spape, J., Timmers, A. D., Yoon, S., Ponseti, J., & Chivers, M. L. (2014). Gender-specific genital and subjective sexual arousal to prepotent sexual features in heterosexual women and men. Biological Psychology, 102, 1–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spector, I. P., Carey, M. P., & Steinberg, L. (1996). The sexual desire inventory: development, factor structure, and evidence of reliability. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 22(3), 175–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spiering, M., Everaerd, W., & Laan, E. (2004). Conscious processing of sexual information: mechanisms of appraisal. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33, 369–380. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:ASEB.0000028890.08687.94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strassberg, D. S., & Lowe, K. (1995). Volunteer bias in sexuality research. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 24(4), 369–382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Twenge, J. M., Sherman, R. A., & Wells, B. E. (2017). Declines in sexual frequency among American adults, 1989–2014. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 1–13.

  • Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., & Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: Individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 103–122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tybur, J. M., Bryan, A. D., Lieberman, D., Hooper, A. E. C., & Merriman, L. A. (2011). Sex differences and sex similarities in disgust sensitivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(3), 343–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Overveld, M., & Borg, C. (2015). Brief emotion regulation training facilitates arousal control during sexual stimuli. The Journal of Sex Research, 52(9), 996–1005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vitzthum, V. J., Bentley, G. R., Spielvogel, H., Caceres, E., Thornburg, J., Jones, L., Shore, S., Hodges, K. R., & Chatterton, R. T. (2002). Salivary progesterone levels and rate of ovulation are significantly lower in poorer than in better-off urban-dwelling Bolivian women. Human Reproduction, 17(7), 1906–1913.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 30, 829–841.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilcox, A. J., Dunson, D. B., Weinberg, C. R., Trussell, J., & Baird, D. D. (2001). Likelihood of conception with a single act of intercourse: providing benchmark rates for assessment of post-coital contraceptives. Contraception, 63(4), 211–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woody, S. R., McLean, C., & Klassen, T. (2005). Disgust as a motivator of avoidance of spiders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19, 461–475.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Żelaźniewicz, A., Borkowska, B., Nowak, J., & Pawłowski, B. (2016). The progesterone level, leukocyte count and disgust sensitivity across the menstrual cycle. Physiology & Behavior, 161, 60–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zsok, F., Fleischman, D. S., Borg, C., & Morrison, E. (2017). Disgust trumps lust: women’s disgust and attraction towards men is unaffected by sexual arousal. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 1–11.

Download references

Acknowledgments

Many thanks are given to the study participants for their invaluable assistance in completing this study.

Funding

This research was supported by grants from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (Grant No. 386198-10) and the Queen’s Senate Advisory Research Council, awarded to M. L. Chivers and an Ontario Women’s Health Scholar Award (funded by the Ministry of Health and Long Term Care) and a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada Joseph Armand Bombardier Canada Graduate Scholarship awarded to the first author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Meredith L. Chivers.

Ethics declarations

Study procedures were approved by our Health Sciences and Affiliated Teaching Hospitals Research Ethics Board. All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Timmers, A.D., Bossio, J.A. & Chivers, M.L. Disgust, Sexual Cues, and the Prophylaxis Hypothesis. Evolutionary Psychological Science 4, 179–190 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-017-0127-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-017-0127-3

Keywords

Navigation