Log in

A Historical Perspective on the Future of Behavior Science

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Like all natural sciences, behavior science has much to offer toward an understanding of the world. The extent to which the promise of behavior science is realized, though, depends upon the extent to which we keep what we know before us. This paper considers fundamental concepts in behavior science, including the concepts of behavior, stimulation, setting conditions, and language. In considering these concepts, we revisit comments from B. F. Skinner and J. R. Kantor and also consider some areas of behavior analytic research and the implications they have for reconsidering long-held assumptions about the analysis of behavior. We hope that, in considering our foundations, the vitality and strength of the discipline might be enhanced, our impact on science improved, and our future secured.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The term “function” here is roughly synonymous with “action” as we have been using this term. It refers to the participation of stimulation in a mutual and reciprocal relation with responding.

  2. Importantly, while we acknowledge that behavior analysts have provided a number of ways to interpret various phenomena (e.g., Hayes et al. 2001; Sidman 2000; Tonneau 2001), Kantor’s conceptualization of stimulus substitution seems to be the most consistent with the aims we have described thus far, and a detailed consideration of the various theories on this topic is far beyond the scope of the current paper.

References

  • Baron, A., Perone, M., & Galizio, M. (1991). Analyzing the reinforcement process at the human level: can application and behavioristic interpretation replace laboratory research? The Behavior Analyst, 14, 95–105.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (2013). What counts as behavior: the molar multiscale view. The Behavior Analyst, 36, 283–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dymond, S., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2000). Understanding complex behavior: the transformation of stimulus functions. The Behavior Analyst, 23, 239–254.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fryling, M. J., & Hayes, L. J. (2009). Psychological events and constructs: an alliance with Smith. The Psychological Record, 59, 133–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fryling, M. J., & Hayes, L. J. (2011). The concept of function in the analysis of behavior. Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 37, 11–20. doi:10.5514/rmac.v37.i1.24686.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L. J. (1992). The psychological present. The Behavior Analyst, 15, 139–145.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L. J. (1998). Remembering as a psychological event. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 18, 135–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L. J. (2001). Finding our place in a constructed future. In L. J. Hayes, J. Austin, R. Houmanfar, & M. C. Clayton (Eds.), Organizational change (pp. 349–372). Reno: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L. J. (2013). Theory and philosophy: future directions. The Behavior Analyst, 36, 373–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L. J., Adams, M., & Dixon, M. R. (1997). Causal constructs and conceptual confusions. The Psychological Record, 46, 97–111.

  • Hayes, L. J., & Delgado, D. (2005). Transgenic and knockout models: the problem of language. In G. Fish & J. Flint (Eds.), Transgenic and knockout models of neuropsychiatric disorders (pp. 45–67). Towanta: Humana.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L. J., & Delgado, D. (2007). Animal models of non-conventional human behavior. Behavior Genetics, 37, 11–17. doi:10.1007/s10519-006-9126-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L. J., & Fryling, M. J. (2009a). Overcoming the pseudo-problem of private events in the analysis of behavior. Behavior and Philosophy, 37, 39–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L. J., & Fryling, M. J. (2009b). Toward an interdisciplinary science of culture. The Psychological Record, 59, 679–700.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: a post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. (1924). Principles of psychology (Vol. I). Bloomington: Principia Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. (1950). Psychology and logic (Vol. II). Chicago: The Principia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. (1953). The logic of modern science. Chicago: Principia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. (1958). Interbehavioral psychology. Chicago: Principia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. (1977). Psychological linguistics. Chicago: The Principia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R., & Smith, N. W. (1975). The science of psychology: an interbehavioral survey. Chicago: The Principia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr, M. J. (2013). Theory and philosophy: Themes and variations. The Behavior Analyst, 36, 193–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parrott, L. J. (1983a). Similarities and differences among Skinner’s Radical Behaviorism and Kantor’s Interbehaviorism. Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 9, 95–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parrott, L. J. (1983b). Systemic foundations for the concept of ‘private events’. In N. W. Smith, P. T. Mountjoy, & D. H. Ruben (Eds.), Reassessment in psychology: the interbehavioral alternative (pp. 251–268). Lanham: University Press of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parrott, L. J. (1984). Listening and understanding. The Behavior Analyst, 7, 29–39.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parrott, L. J. (1986). On the role of postulation in the analysis of inapparent events. In H. W. Reese & L. J. Parrott (Eds.), Behavior science: philosophical, methodological, and empirical advances (pp. 35–60). Hillsdale: L. Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H. (2013). About teleological behaviorism. The Behavior Analyst, 36, 209–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (2000). Equivalence relations and the reinforcement contingency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 127–146. doi:10.1901/jeab.2000.74-127.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Croft.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tonneau, F. (2001). Equivalence relations: a critical analysis. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 2, 1–33.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Linda J. Hayes.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hayes, L.J., Fryling, M.J. A Historical Perspective on the Future of Behavior Science. BEHAV ANALYST 38, 149–161 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-015-0030-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-015-0030-9

Keywords

Navigation