Log in

Building Blocks of Psychology: on Remaking the Unkept Promises of Early Schools

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The appeal and popularity of “building blocks”, i.e., simple and dissociable elements of behavior and experience, persists in psychological research. We begin our assessment of this research strategy with an historical review of structuralism (as espoused by E. B. Titchener) and behaviorism (espoused by J. B. Watson and B. F. Skinner), two movements that held the assumption in their attempts to provide a systematic and unified discipline. We point out the ways in which the elementism of the two schools selected, framed, and excluded topics of study. After the historical review, we turn to contemporary literature and highlight the persistence of research into building blocks and the associated framing and exclusions in psychological research. The assumption that complex categories of human psychology can be understood in terms of their elementary components and simplest forms seems indefensible. In specific cases, therefore, reliance on the assumption requires justification. Finally, we review alternative strategies that bypass the commitment to building blocks.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price includes VAT (Germany)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We refrain from using “modularity” and “faculty psychology” due to association of these terms with specific debates, e.g., the degree of separation between perception and thought (Fodor 1983). The term “elementism” seems more suitable for the present discussion, both for its broader applicability and for its close association to reductionism (Danziger 1979).

  2. Green et al. (2013, 2014) characterize this separation in terms of diverging genres of writing during late-19th and early-twentieth century. Klempe (2015) traces the separation to Early Modern period and the fundamental tension between empirical psychology (which confronts particularity), and metaphysics (which aims to achieve universality).

  3. In using the term “structuralism”, we refer exclusively to the psychological approach advocated by Titchener, and not the approach to linguistics, and later more broadly to social sciences and humanities, which originated in the work of Ferdinand de Saussure.

  4. Araujo and Marcellos (2017) and Beenfeldt (2013) trace this aspect of Titchener’s structuralism to the British philosophical movements of his time. Also see Billig (2008), who examines the influence of British Empiricism in psychological thinking.

  5. It is helpful to remember how Titchener distanced himself from Brentano’s project (Titchener 1921a). A characteristic trait of Brentano’s psychology was his emphasis on directedness (or about-ness) of conscious states, which recognizes their functional significance (Brentano 1874; see also, Fasching 2012; Tassone 2012). Changing the meaning of objects of consciousness, in Brentano’s view, is not only a change in the quality of experience, but it is a functional change that can be expressed in behavior.

  6. William James (1892, pp. 18-19), among others, contrasted the two approaches in terms of their starting points. The approach favored by James begins with identifying a complex process and analyzing its attributes, whereas the elementist approach begins with an artificially simple process – chosen on theoretical grounds – and proceeds to augment it incrementally.

References

  • Abrahamse, E., Braem, S., Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2016). Grounding cognitive control in associative learning. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 693–728.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abrams, R. A., & Weidler, B. J. (2014). Trade-offs in visual processing for stimuli near the hands. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 383–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abrams, R. A., Davoli, C. C., Du, F., Knapp, W. H., & Paull, D. (2008). Altered vision near the hands. Cognition, 107, 1035–1047.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Angell, J. R. (1907). The province of functional psychology. Psychological Review, 14, 61–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Angell, J. R. (1913). Behavior as a category of psychology. Psychological Review, 20, 255–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Araujo, S. F. (2013). Searle's new mystery, or, how not to solve the problem of consciousness. Rivista Internazionale di Filosofia e Psicologia, 4, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Araujo, S. D. F., & Marcellos, C. F. (2017). From classicism and idealism to scientific naturalism: Titchener’s Oxford years and their impact upon his early intellectual development. History of Psychology, 20, 148–171.

  • Awad, S. H., Wagoner, B., & Glaveanu, V. (2017). The Street Art of Resistance. In N. Chaudhary, P. Hviid, G. Marsico, & J. W. Villadsen (Eds.), Resistance in Everyday Life (pp. 161–180, Springer). Singapore.

  • Awh, E., Barton, B., & Vogel, E. K. (2007). Visual working memory represents a fixed number of items regardless of complexity. Psychological Science, 18, 622–628.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ayçiçeği, A., & Harris, C. L. (2004). Bilinguals' recall and recognition of emotion words. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 977–987.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J. (1986). The cognitive revolution in psychology. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J. (1997). Contrastive phenomenology: A thoroughly empirical approach to consciousness. In N. Block, O. Flanagan, & G. Güzeldere (Eds.), The Nature of Consciousness: Philosophical Controversies (pp. 187–202). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Masicampo, E. J., & Vohs, K. D. (2011). Do conscious thoughts cause behavior? Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 311–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, S. I. (2010). The role of target–distractor relationships in guiding attention and the eyes in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 247–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, S. I., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2013). Attentional capture does not depend on feature similarity, but on target-nontarget relations. Psychological Science, 24, 634–647.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beenfeldt, C. (2013). The Philosophical Background and Scientific Legacy of E.B. Titchener's Psychology: Understanding Introspectionism. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bergner, R. M. (2016). What is behaviour? And why is it not reducible to biological states of affairs? Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 36, 41–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bergner, R. M. (2017). What is a person? What is the self? Formulations for a science of psychology. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 37, 77–90.

  • Billig, M. (2008). The hidden roots of critical psychology: Understanding the impact of Locke, Shaftesbury and Reid. London: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackman, L. (2014). Affect and automaticity: Towards an analytics of experimentation. Subjectivity, 7, 362–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (1995). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 227–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (2005). Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 46–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bocanegra, B. R., & Zeelenberg, R. (2011). Emotion-induced trade-offs in spatiotemporal vision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 272–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brady, T. F., Konkle, T., & Alvarez, G. A. (2011). A review of visual memory capacity: Beyond individual items and toward structured representations. Journal of Vision, 11, 4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brentano, F. (1874). Psychologie vom empirischen Standpunkt. Hamburg: Meiner. Engl. transl. (1973). Psychology from an Empirical Standpoint (trans: Rancurello, A. C., Terrell, D. B. & McAlister, L. L.). New York: Humanities.

  • Bush, W. S., & Vecera, S. P. (2014). Differential effect of one versus two hands on visual processing. Cognition, 133, 232–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M. A., & Dennett, D. C. (2011). Consciousness cannot be separated from function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 358–364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Constable, M. D., Pratt, J., Gozli, D. G., & Welsh, T. N. (2015). Do you see what I see: Co-actor posture modulates visual processing in joint tasks. Visual Cognition, 23, 699–719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cosman, J. D., & Vecera, S. P. (2010). Attention Affects Visual Perceptual Processing Near the Hand. Psychological Science, 21, 1254–1258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1979). The positivist repudiation of Wundt. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 15, 205–230.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1980). The history of introspection reconsidered. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 16, 241–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davoli, C. C., Du, F., Montana, J., Garverick, S., & Abrams, R. A. (2010). When meaning matters, look but don't touch: The effects of posture on reading. Memory & Cognition, 38, 555–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2011). Why the cognitive approach in psychology would profit from a functional approach and vice versa. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 202–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Depraz, N., Gyemant, M., & Desmidt, T. (2017). A first-person analysis using third-person data as a generative method: A case study of surprise in depression. Constructivist Foundations, 12, 190–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1896). The reflex arc concept in psychology. Psychological Review, 3, 357–370.

  • Diriwächter, R., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.). (2008). Striving for the whole. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dukewich, K. R., & Klein, R. M. (2015). Inhibition of return: A phenomenon in search of a definition and a theoretical framework. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 1647–1658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96, 433–458.

  • Eitam, B., Shoval, R., & Yeshurun, Y. (2015). Seeing without knowing: task relevance dissociates between visual awareness and recognition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1339, 125–137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Engeström, Y. (2011). From design experiments to formative interventions. Theory & Psychology, 21, 598–628.

  • Evans, R. B. (1972). E. B. Titchener and his lost system. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 8, 168–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farrell, M. (2014). Historical and philosophical foundations of psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fasching, W. (2012). Intentionality and presence: On the intrinsic of-ness of consciousness from a transcendental-phenomenological perspective. Husserl Studies, 28, 121–141.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fei-Fei, L., VanRullen, R., Koch, C., & Perona, P. (2005). Why does natural scene recognition require little attention? Exploring attentional requirements for natural and synthetic stimuli. Visual Cognition, 12, 893–924.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festman, Y., Adam, J. J., Pratt, J., & Fischer, M. H. (2013). Both hand position and movement direction modulate visual attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 657.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts, P. M., & Seeger, C. M. (1953). SR compatibility: spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 199–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fougnie, D., Asplund, C. L., & Marois, R. (2010). What are the units of storage in visual working memory? Journal of Vision, 10, 27.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ganel, T., & Goodale, M. A. (2003). Visual control of action but not perception requires analytical processing of object shape. Nature, 426, 664–667.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J. (2004). Why is conversation so easy? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 8–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geipel, J., Hadjichristidis, C., & Surian, L. (2015). How foreign language shapes moral judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 59, 8–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geipel, J., Hadjichristidis, C., & Surian, L. (2016). Foreign language affects the contribution of intentions and outcomes to moral judgment. Cognition, 154, 34–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giorgi, A. (2012). The descriptive phenomenological psychological method. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 43, 3–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giorgi, A. (2014). Phenomenological philosophy as the basis for a human scientific psychology. Humanistic Psychologist, 42, 233–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodhew, S. C., & Clarke, R. (2016). Contributions of the parvocellular and magnocellular pathways to visual perception near the hands are not fixed, but can be dynamically altered. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 156–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodhew, S. C., Visser, T. A., Lipp, O. V., & Dux, P. E. (2011). Implicit semantic perception in object substitution masking. Cognition, 118, 130–134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodhew, S. C., Gozli, D. G., Ferber, S., & Pratt, J. (2013). Reduced temporal fusion in the near-hand space. Psychological Science, 24, 891–900.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gozli, D. G. (2017). Behaviour versus performance: the veiled commitment of experimental psychology. Theory & Psychology. Online first. doi:10.1177/0959354317728130

  • Gozli, D. G., West, G. L., & Pratt, J. (2012). Hand position alters vision by biasing processing through different visual pathways. Cognition, 124, 244–250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gozli, D. G., Ardron, J., & Pratt, J. (2014). Reduced visual feature binding in the near-hand space. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 1308–1317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, C. D. (2010). Scientific objectivity and E.B. Titchener's experimental psychology. Isis, 101(4), 697–721.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, C. D. (2015). Why psychology isn't unified, and probably never will be. Review of General Psychology, 19, 207–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, C. D., Feinerer, I., & Burman, J. T. (2013). Beyond the schools of psychology 1: A digital analysis of Psychological Review, 1894–1903. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 49, 167–189.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, C. D., Feinerer, I., & Burman, J. T. (2014). Beyond the schools of psychology 2: A digital analysis of Psychological Review, 1904–1923. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 50, 249–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gurwitsch, A. (2009). Some aspects and developments of Gestalt psychology. F. Kersten (Ed.), The Collected Works of Aron Gurwitsch (1901–1973), Volume II: Studies in Phenomenology and Psychology (pp. 1–61). Dordrecht: Springer. Originally published (1936) in Journal de Psychologie Normal et Pathologique.

  • Hacker, P. M. S. (2015). Philosophy and scientism: What cognitive neuroscience can, and what it cannot, explain. In D. N. Robinson & R. N. Williams (Eds.), Scientism: The New Orthodoxy. London: Bloomsbury Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hari, R., & Jousmäki, V. (1996). Preference of personal to extrapersonal space in a visuomotor task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 305–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayakawa, S., Costa, A., Foucart, A., & Keysar, B. (2016). Using a foreign language changes our choices. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 791–793.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herwig, A., Beisert, M., & Prinz, W. (2013). Action science emerging: Introduction and leitmotifs. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action Science: Foundations of an Emerging Discipline (pp. 1–33). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hibberd, F. J. (2014). The metaphysical basis of a process psychology. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 34, 161–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hibberd, F. J. (2016). Is conceptual analysis only an inquiry into rules for the use of concepts? Theory & Psychology, 26, 815–822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann, J., Stock, A., & Deutsch, R. (1996). The Wurzburg School. In J. Hoffmann & A. Sebald (Eds.), Cognitive psychology in Europe: proceedings of the ninth conference of the European society for cognitive psychology (pp. 147–172). Lengerich: Pabst Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, 55, 270–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (2000). The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 247–273). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (2015). Between persistence and flexibility: The Yin and Yang of action control. In A. J. Elliot (Ed.), Advances in Motivation Science (Vol. 2, pp. 33–67). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. S. (2015). Learning from history: The need for a synthetic approach to human cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1435.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Lippelt, D. P., Gurbuz, E., & Pfister, R. (2016). Contributions of expected sensory and affective action effects to action selection and performance: evidence from forced- and free-choice tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24, 821–827.

  • Huffman, G., Gozli, D. G., Welsh, T. N., & Pratt, J. (2015). Hand position influences perceptual grou**. Experimental Brain Research, 233, 2627–2634.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1892). Psychology: The briefer course. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Javier, R. A., Barroso, F., & Muñoz, M. A. (1993). Autobiographical memory in bilinguals. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 22, 319–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jusyte, A., Pfister, R., Mayer, S. V., Schwarz, K. A., Wirth, R., Kunde, W., & Schönenberg, M. (2017). Smooth criminal: convicted rule-breakers show reduced cognitive conflict during deliberate rule violations. Psychological Research, 81, 939–946.

  • Kagan, J. (2012). Psychology's ghosts: The crisis in the profession and the way back. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kattsoff, L. O. (1948). What is behavior? Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 9, 98–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, S. P., & Brockmole, J. R. (2014). Hand proximity differentially affects visual working memory for color and orientation in a binding task. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 318.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Keysar, B., Hayakawa, S. L., & An, S. G. (2012). The foreign-language effect thinking in a foreign tongue reduces decision biases. Psychological Science, 23, 661–668.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kingstone, A., Smilek, D., & Eastwood, J. D. (2008). Cognitive ethology: A new approach for studying human cognition. British Journal of Psychology, 99, 317–340.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klempe, S. H. (2012). Psychology – tensions between objectivity and subjectivity. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 46, 373–379.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klempe, S. H. (2015). Thinking in early modernity and the separation process between philosophy and psychology. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 49, 44–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kordeš, U. (2016). Going beyond theory: Constructivism and empirical phenomenology. Constructivist Foundations, 11, 375–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kukla, A., & Walmsley, J. (2006). Mind: A Historical and Philosophical Introduction to the Major Theories. Indianapolis: Hackett Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamiell, J. T. (2007). On sustaining critical discourse with mainstream personality investigators: Problems and prospects. Theory & Psychology, 17, 169–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamiell, J. T. (2013). On psychology’s struggle for existence: Some reflections on Wundt’s 1913 essay a century on. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 33, 205–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamy, D., Antebi, C., Aviani, N., & Carmel, T. (2008). Priming of pop-out provides reliable measures of target activation and distractor inhibition in selective attention. Vision Research, 48, 30–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leahey, T. H. (1981). The mistaken mirror: On Wundt's and Titchener's psychologies. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 17, 273–282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Li, F. F., VanRullen, R., Koch, C., & Perona, P. (2002). Rapid natural scene categorization in the near absence of attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 99, 9601–9596.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maljkovic, V., & Nakayama, K. (1994). Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features. Memory & Cognition, 22, 657–672.

  • Mammen, J. (1993). The elements of psychology. In N. Engelsted, M. Hedegaard, B. Karpatschof, & A. Mortensen (Eds.), The Societal Subject (pp. 29–44). Aarhus: Aarhus University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marken, R. S. (1988). The nature of behavior: Control as fact and theory. Behavioral Science, 33, 196–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marken, R. S. (2002). Looking at behavior through control theory glasses. Review of General Psychology, 6, 260–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marken, R. S. (2009). You say you had a revolution: Methodological foundations of closed-loop psychology. Review of General Psychology, 13, 137–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marken, R. S. (2013). Taking purpose into account in experimental psychology: Testing for controlled variables. Psychological Reports, 112, 184–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meiser, T. (2011). Much pain, little gain? Paradigm-specific models and methods in experimental psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 183–191.

  • Mele, A. R. (1992). Springs of action: Understanding intentional behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Memelink, J., & Hommel, B. (2013). Intentional weighting: A basic principle in cognitive control. Psychological Research, 77, 249–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moeller, B., Frings, C., & Pfister, R. (2016). The structure of distractor-response bindings. Conditions for configural and elemental integration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 464–479.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morsella, E., Godwin, C. A., Jantz, T. J., Krieger, S. C., & Gazzaley, A. (2016). Homing in on consciousness in the nervous system: An action-based synthesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. (1973). You can't play 20 questions with nature and win: Projective comments on the papers of this symposium. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual Information Processing. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nie, Q. Y., Müller, H. J., & Conci, M. (2017). Hierarchical organization in visual working memory: From global ensemble to individual object structure. Cognition, 159, 85–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Noë, A. (2012). Varieties of presence. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • O'Boyle, C. G. (2006). History of Psychology: A Cultural Perspective. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pacherie, E. (2008). The phenomenology of action: A conceptual framework. Cognition, 107, 179–217.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perler, D.(2015), (Ed.). The faculties: A history. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Persaud, N., & Cowey, A. (2008). Blindsight is unlike normal conscious vision: Evidence from an exclusion task. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 1050–1055.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pfister, R., Janczyk, M., Wirth, R., Dignath, D., & Kunde, W. (2014). Thinking with portals: Revisiting kinematic cues to intention. Cognition, 133, 464–473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pfister, R., Wirth, R., Schwarz, K., Steinhauser, M., & Kunde, W. (2016). Burdens of non-conformity: Motor execution reveals cognitive conflict during deliberate rule violations. Cognition, 147, 93–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pillsbury, W. B. (1928). The psychology of Edward Bradford Titchener. Philosophical Review, 37, 95–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, J. R., & Portillo, M. C. (2011). Grou** and emergent features in vision: toward a theory of basic Gestalts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1331–1349.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Powers, W. T. (1998). Making sense of behavior. Montclair: Benchmark Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, C. L., Grubb, J. D., & Steele, C. (2006). Hands up: attentional prioritization of space near the hand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 166–177.

  • Richardson, F. C. (2000). Overcoming fragmentation in psychology: A hermeneutic approach. The Journal of Mind and Behavior, 21, 289–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Risko, E. F., Richardson, D. C., & Kingstone, A. (2016). Breaking the fourth wall of cognitive science real-world social attention and the dual function of gaze. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 70–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ristic, J., & Kingstone, A. (2006). Attention to arrows: Pointing to a new direction. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1921–1930.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. N. (1995). An Intellectual History of Psychology (3rd ed.). Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. N. (2016). Explanation and the "brain sciences". Theory & Psychology, 26, 324–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sampson, E. E. (1981). Cognitive psychology as ideology. American Psychologist, 36, 730–743.

  • Skinner, B. F. (1984). Selection by consequences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 477–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1987). Whatever happened to psychology as the science of behavior? American Psychologist, 42, 780–786.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1989). The origins of cognitive thought. American Psychologist, 44, 13–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. (2012). The bricoleur model of psychological practice. Theory & Psychology, 22, 643–657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. (2016). Why psychology cannot be an empirical science. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 50, 185–195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, L. D. (1992). On prediction and control: B.F. Skinner and the technological ideal of science. American Psychologist, 47, 216–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stengers, I. (2008). Experimenting with refrains: Subjectivity and the challenge of esca** modern dualism. Subjectivity, 22, 38–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stenner, P. (2008). A.N. Whitehead and subjectivity. Subjectivity, 22, 90–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suh, J., & Abrams, R. A. (2015). Reduced object-based perception in the near-hand space. Experimental Brain Research, 233, 3403–3412.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tassone, B. (2012). From Psychology to Phenomenology: Franz Brentano's 'Psychology from an Empirical Standpoint' and Contemporary Philosophy of Mind. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tawney, G. A. (1915). What is behavior? The Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Method, 12, 29–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, L. E. (2015). Grasp posture alters visual processing biases near the hands. Psychological Science, 26, 625–632.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, T., & Sunny, M. M. (2017). Slower attentional disengagement but faster perceptual processing near the hand. Acta Psychologica, 174, 40–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1934). Primitive forms of belief and knowledge. Psychological Review, 41, 403–411.

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1946). Expectation. Psychological Review, 53, 277–281.

  • Titchener, E. B. (1898). The postulates of a structural psychology. Philosophical Review, 7, 449–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. (1905). The problems of experimental psychology. The American Journal of Psychology, 16, 208–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. (1912). Description vs. statement of meaning. The American Journal of Psychology, 23, 165–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. (1921a). Brentano and Wundt: Empirical and experimental psychology. The American Journal of Psychology, 32, 108–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. (1921b). Functional psychology and the psychology of act: I. The American Journal of Psychology, 32, 519–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2016). What are higher psychological functions? Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 50, 91–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., & Sato, S. (1990). Conjunction search revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 459–478.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2009). Integrating Psychology within the globalizing world: A requiem to the post-modernist experiment with wissenschaft. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 43, 1–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2012). A guided science: History of psychology in the mirror of its making. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagoner, B. (2011). What happened to holism? Psychological Studies, 56, 318–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagoner, B., & Brescó, I. (2016). Conflict and memory: The past in the present. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 22, 3–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wapner, S. (1964). Some aspects of a research program based on an organismic-developmental approach to cognition: Experiments and theory. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 3, 193–230.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1916). The place of the conditioned-reflex in psychology. Psychological Review, 23, 89–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1917). An Attempted formulation of the scope of behavior psychology. Psychological Review, 24, 329–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1920). Is thinking merely action of the language mechanisms? British Journal of Psychology, 11, 87–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H., & Wapner, S. (1952). Toward a general theory of perception. Psychological Review, 59, 324–338.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer, M. (2012). A Brief History of Psychology (5th ed.). New York: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, R. H. (1925). Persistent problems in systematic psychology. I. Philosophical heritage. Psychological Review, 32, 179–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, R. H. (1928). Persistent Problems in Systematic Psychology IV. Structural versus Functional Analysis. The Journal of General Psychology, 1, 91–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiemers, M., & Fischer, M. H. (2016). Effects of hand proximity and movement direction in spatial and temporal gap discrimination. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1930.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wirth, R., Pfister, R., Foerster, A., Huestegge, L., & Kunde, W. (2016). Pushing the rules: Effects and aftereffects of deliberate rule violations. Psychological Research, 80, 838–852.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, D. (2007). Killing the straw man: Dennett and phenomenology. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6, 21–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zittoun, T. (2016). Living creatively, in and through institutions. Europe's Journal of Psychology, 12, 1–11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zittoun, T. (2017). Modalities of generalization through single case studies. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 51, 171–194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are thankful to Jaan Valsiner, Hroar Klempe, Mark Solovey, and two anonymous reviewers for feedback on earlier drafts.

Funding

This work was supported by internal Start-up Research Grants (SRG2016), from University of Macau, awarded to Davood Gozli and Wei Deng.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Davood G. Gozli.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

Davood Gozli declares that he has no conflict of interest. Wei Deng declares that she has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

This article does not contain any studies with human participants or animals performed by any of the authors.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gozli, D.G., Deng, W.(. Building Blocks of Psychology: on Remaking the Unkept Promises of Early Schools. Integr. psych. behav. 52, 1–24 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-017-9405-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-017-9405-7

Keywords

Navigation