Playing videogames has become one of the most popular entertainment events around the world, especially since the inception of online multiplayer games featuring both collaboration and competition (Nuyens et al. 2016). Videogames are often an enjoyable way of reducing stress, socializing with friends, and spending time (Stockdale and Coyne 2018). Through playing videogames, many goals are achieved such as relaxation, competence, autonomy, and avoidance of daily concerns (Griffiths 2003; Ryan et al. 2006). Due to the “flow” status of playing games, the player gets a sense of control, loses sense of time and place, and finds playing self-rewarding (Csikszentmihalyi 1990). Greater ventral striatal presynaptic dopamine release in healthy volunteers during game-playing supports the assumption that the activity is potentially rewarding in itself (Koepp et al. 1998). After a 6-week gaming period, when the gaming cues were encountered, increased orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate activity was observed in healthy volunteers. This suggests that game-playing can be reinforcing and associated cues can become conditioned reinforcers (Han et al. 2010). For most videogame players, the activity is fun and exciting but when it harms the player’s social, occupational/educational, family, and psychological functioning, it can become pathological (Gentile et al. 2011). In other words, for this small minority of individuals, they become preoccupied with playing games constantly, deprive themselves of their social life for the sake of playing, and use gaming to cope with real-world pressure, all of which can be symptoms of addiction (Gunuc 2015; Kuss and Griffiths 2012; Mai et al. 2012).

The psychological mechanisms underlying the prevention and intervention of problematic gaming needs to be understood (Hu et al. 2017). More specifically, many issues need to be clarified in order for Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD) to be considered as a formal diagnosis in the next edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Such issues include risk and protective factors for IGD, comorbid patterns with IGD, structure of IGD, typical consequences of IGD, and effective approaches in overcoming IGD (Gentile et al. 2011).

Behavioral patterns such as persistent thoughts related with online gaming and continuous use of the internet to take part in games are basis of the clinical diagnosis of IGD. In extreme cases, these patterns lead to serious impairment or distress (American Psychiatric Association 2013). Consequently, IGD has been defined and conceptualized in the context of behavioral addiction (Dowling 2014; Pontes et al. 2014) and the need to determine the diagnostic criteria for the differentiating IGD from gambling and substance addictions has been emphasized by many researchers (Griffiths et al. 2016). Several modified criteria used for substance addiction have been utilized for IGD in the DSM-5. In addition to the aforementioned criteria, other criteria comprising IGD include tolerance, withdrawal, persistent use despite negative outcomes, inability to restrict internet use, disruption in psychosocial functioning, loss of interest in previous hobbies, and escapism (Yen et al. 2017).

Both positive reinforcement (i.e., receiving a reward or reaching higher levels in the videogame) and negative reinforcement (i.e., playing to avoid negative affectivity) can be factors in the development of gaming addiction (Choi et al. 2007; Yee 2006). Other risk factors for IGD include decreased social competence (Gentile et al. 2011), poor impulse control (Griffiths et al. 2012), increased time allocated to gaming (Gentile et al. 2011), sensation seeking, narcissistic personality traits (Griffiths et al. 2012), and high anxiety (Mehroof and Griffiths 2010). Furthermore, distortion in response inhibition when faced with the game-related clues has also been associated with gaming addiction. Here, attentional biases may be effective in maintaining the problem by increasing the risk of relapse (van Holst et al. 2012).

Repetitive harmful behavior and loss of control have been proposed as specific features of IGD in both the DSM-5 and ICD-11 (Király and Demetrovics 2017). A variety of executive control problems are associated with IGD, such as failure in response inhibition (Ding et al. 2014), disrupted error monitoring (Dong et al. 2013), and cognitive inflexibility (Dong et al. 2014). It is also known that high-level impulsivity is related with IGD (Cao et al. 2007). Studies evaluating neurocognitive functions have proposed that impulsivity in IGD is more pronounced compared to other behavioral addictions such as gambling disorder (Choi et al. 2014). Consequently, increased impulsivity is included in most IGD models such as Dual System Models (Bechara 2005) and the Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) Model (Brand et al. 2016). According to neuroimaging studies, distortion in the system of cognitive control causes impulsivity (Steinberg et al. 2008) and decreased executive control can be critical in the development of addictive behaviors (Brand et al. 2016). Studies have also demonstrated that during impulse control-related tasks, addicted players experience abnormal activations in frontal, insular, temporal, and parietal cortex compared to healthy controls (Dong et al. 2012). Furthermore, impulsivity is associated with depression (Granö et al. 2007; Swann et al. 2008) and indirectly causes loneliness and failures in interpersonal relationships (Savci and Aysan 2016). Thus, impulsivity can be a contributory factor in consequent emotional and social functioning (Ryu et al. 2018).

Impulsivity is accepted as multidimensional and subdivided into five dimensions encompassing negative and positive urgency (acting rashly when experiencing intense negative or positive emotions respectively), lack of premeditation (acting without considering the consequences), lack of perseverance (failure in maintaining focus on a boring or difficult task), and sensation seeking (pursuing exciting activities) (Cyders and Smith 2008). In other words, impulsivity is associated with inability to consider alternative solutions, doing the first thing that comes to mind without assessing the consequences (D’Zurilla et al. 2003), and failure in self-control when faced with reward or punishment (Stewart et al. 2004). Thus, impulsivity is recognized as a predictive factor in the development of maladaptive behaviors resulting in relief from negative emotions (Cyders and Smith 2008). Despite such research, to the present authors’ knowledge, there has never been a review paper examining the relationship between impulsivity and IGD utilizing the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines. Consequently, the present paper synthesizes the available literature according to PRISMA guidelines.

Method

Eligibility Criteria

All studies assessing the phenomenon of IGD and impulsivity were eligible for review. The inclusion criteria were (i) publication date between 2000 and 2019, (ii) being an empirical study that collected primary data, (iii) written in English and Turkish languages (the two languages spoken by the authors), (iv) published in a scholarly peer-reviewed journal, and (v) conducted an objective assessment of both IGD and impulsivity. Studies were excluded from the review if they were (i) single-case studies, (ii) unpublished thesis and dissertation studies, and (iii) not published in a peer-reviewed journal.

Information Sources and Search

A literature search was conducted via EBSCO (which included the following academic databases: Academic Search Complete, PsycARTICLES, and PsycINFO), PubMed, ScienceDirect, Web of Science, and Wiley Online Library. Several searches in the stated electronic databases were conducted during February 2019 using the following search terms: (internet OR video OR computer) AND game OR gaming (addict* OR depend* OR disorder) AND impulsivity OR impulse control OR impuls*.

Study Selection and Data Collection Processes

After the initial literature searches that were conducted, the title and abstract of each study was screened and then potentially relevant studies were further evaluated for eligibility. The detailed information about study selection process is provided in the PRISMA flow diagram (Fig. 1). In the process of assessing risk of bias in each study, sampling bias and measurement bias were also assessed.

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA flow diagram of paper selection process used in the present study

Results

Study Selection

A total of 1663 studies (EBSCO, n = 581; PubMed, n = 286; ScienceDirect, n = 442; Web of Science, n = 301; Wiley Online Library, n = 53) were identified in the initial search process. After the title and abstract of each study was examined, 1612 studies were excluded because of unsuitability for the present review. A further 12 studies were excluded due to duplication (n = 11) or only having an abstract (n = 1). Consequently, a total of 39 studies were selected for eligibility phase. Of these, a total of six studies were excluded for language issues (n = 1), being an unpublished dissertation (n = 2), assessing the overall game playing time as dependent variable (n = 1), or not assessing IGD objectively (n = 2). Following these procedures, 33 eligible empirical studies remained for evaluation in the present review.

Study Characteristics

Information about the general characteristics and main methodological properties of all 33 studies can be found in Tables 1 and 2.

Table 1 Main characteristics of the studies reviewed (authors, sample size, gender distribution, age range, mean age, and sample characteristics (N = 33)
Table 2 Main findings, study limitations, and risk of biases in the studies reviewed (N = 33)

Country in Which the Data Were Collected

Regarding the geographic characteristics of the included studies, nine were from China (Ding et al. 2014; Du et al. 2016; Du et al. 2017; Hu et al. 2017; Tian et al. 2018; Wang et al. 2016,2017; Wu et al. 2018; Yao et al. 2017), eight were from South Korea (Choi et al. 2014; Hyun et al. 2015; Kim et al. 2017a; Kim et al. 2017b; Lee et al. 2017; Lee et al. 2017; Rho et al. 2017; Ryu et al. 2018), three were from Germany (Rehbein et al. 2010; Sariyska et al. 2017; Walther et al. 2012), two were from Australia (Bailey et al. 2013; Metcalf and Pammer 2014), two were from Singapore (Gentile et al. 2011; Liau et al. 2015), two were from the USA (Bargeron and Hormes 2017; Stockdale and Coyne 2018), one was from Spain (Irles and Gomis 2016), one was from the UK (Irvine et al. 2013), one was from Belgium (Deleuze et al. 2017), one was from the Czech Republic (Blinka et al. 2016), and one was from Taiwan (Yen et al. 2017). Additionally, there were two cross-cultural studies. The first one collected data from France, Switzerland, and Belgium (Billieux et al. 2015), the second collected data from Belgium and the UK (Nuyens et al. 2016).

Participants

The 33 reviewed studies comprised 18,128 participants in total. The vast majority of studies recruited more male participants (n = 12,492; 70.1%) than female participants (n = 5333; 29.9%). No information was provided about the participants’ gender in two studies (Bargeron and Hormes 2017; Yao et al. 2017) in order to avoid the activation of gender-related stereotypes (Bargeron and Hormes 2017). Twenty-four studies included adult samples and nine studies included adolescent samples (Ding et al. 2014; Du et al. 2016; Du et al. 2017; Irles and Gomis 2016; Hu et al. 2017; Liau et al. 2015; Rehbein et al. 2010; Tian et al. 2018; Walther et al. 2012). Eleven studies comprised student samples (Bailey et al. 2013; Gentile et al. 201; Hu et al. 2017; Irles and Gomis 2016; Liau et al. 2015; Rehbein et al. 2010; Sariyska et al. 2017; Stockdale and Coyne 2018; Walther et al. 2012; Wang et al. 2014 ;Wang et al. 2017); nine studies comprised outpatients (Choi et al. 2014; Ding et al. 2014; Du et al. 2017; Hyun et al. 2015; Kim et al. 2017a; Kim et al. 2017b; Lee et al. 2017; Ryu et al. 2018; Tian et al. 2018); and seven studies comprised only gamers (Bargeron and Hormes 2017; Billieux et al. 2015; Blinka et al. 2016; Deleuze et al. 2017; Hu et al. 2017; Irles and Gomis 2016; Nuyens et al. 2016). Of the seven studies comprising gamers only (seven studies included gamers and non-gamers), two studies (Billieux et al. 2015; Nuyens et al. 2016) comprised Massively-Multiplayer Online Role-Playing Games (MMORPGs) players. In addition, seven studies included both gamers and healthy volunteers (Du et al. 2016; Irvine et al. 2013; Lee et al. 2017; Metcalf and Pammer 2014; Wu et al. 2018; Yao et al. 2017; Yen et al. 2017).

Operationalization of Internet Gaming Disorder

Operationalization of a variable comprises describing how a variable is assessed objectively (Dantzker and Hunter 2011). To assess IGD, 15 of the studies the adopted Internet Addiction Test, Young’s Internet Addiction Scale, Chen Internet Addiction Scale, or the Internet Addiction Survey (Billieux et al. 2015; Choi et al. 2014; Ding et al. 2014; Du et al. 2016; Du et al. 2017; Hyun et al. 2015; Kim et al. 2017a; Kim et al. 2017b; Lee et al. 2017, 2018; Rho et al. 2017; Ryu et al. 2018; Tian et al. 2018; Wang et al. 2016,2017; Wu et al. 2018; Yao et al. 2017). Of these 15 studies, half of the studies also assessed IGD using DSM-5 diagnostic criterion items to strengthen the assessment (Choi et al. 2014; Hyun et al. 2015; Kim et al. 2017a; Lee et al. 2018; Rho et al. 2017; Wang et al. 2016,2017; Wu et al. 2018; Yao et al. 2017). In addition, three studies used the Addiction-Engagement Scale which is modified tool based on addiction and engagement portions of General Computing Questionnaire (this scale originally adapted from Computer Apathy and Anxiety Scale) (Blinka et al. 2016; Gentile et al. 2011; Metcalf and Pammer 2014). Two studies used a modified tool based on generalized internet addiction criteria such as the Online Gaming Addiction Scale (Hu et al. 2017; Sariyska et al. 2017). Two studies used the Video Game Dependency Scale which is a modified tool based on non-substance–related addiction according to ICD-10 criteria (Rehbein et al. 2010; Walther et al. 2012). Two studies used the Pathological Gaming Scale, a ten-item instrument derived from the pathological gambling items of the DSM-IV (Bailey et al. 2013; Liau et al. 2015). The remaining seven studies assessed IGD based on gaming addiction/dependency criteria and/or used scales specifically designed to assess problematic gaming (Bargeron and Hormes 2017; Deleuze et al. 2017; Irles and Gomis 2016; Irvine et al. 2013; Nuyens et al. 2016; Stockdale and Coyne 2018; Yen et al. 2017).

The Relationships Between Impulsivity and Internet Gaming Disorder

All number of studies examined the relationship between impulsivity and IGD. Apart from one study which found no relationship (Deleuze et al. 2017), all remaining studies reported that impulsivity was positively correlated with IGD. When the results of regression analysis related with the impulsivity were evaluated, impulsivity was recognized as a predictive factor for IGD in seven studies (Blinka et al. 2016; Gentile et al. 2011; Hu et al. 2017; Hyun et al. 2015; Liau et al. 2015; Rehbein et al. 2010; Ryu et al. 2018). In addition, while self-regulation (Liau et al. 2015), interpersonal relationships, and depression (Ryu et al. 2018) mediated the association between impulsivity and gaming addiction, impulsivity moderated the relationship between positive affective associations and gaming addiction (Hu et al. 2017). Also, dysfunctions of the brain areas involved in behavior inhibition, attention, and emotion regulation were identified as contributing factors for impulse control problems among adolescents with IGD (Ding et al. 2014; Du et al. 2016,2017).

Risk of Bias in Individual Studies

With regard to the process of assessing risk of bias in each study, sampling bias and measurement bias were assessed. On these biases, apart from seven studies (Ding et al. 2014; Hyun et al. 2015; Kim et al. 2017a; Kim et al. 2017b; Rehbein et al. 2010; Rho et al. 2017; Tian et al. 2018), all the remaining 26 studies were rated as having risk of sampling bias due to (i) adopting non-probability sampling techniques and/or self-selected samples, (ii) omission of female samples, and/or (iii) omission of clinical samples. Additionally, measurement bias was rated as high risk in most of the studies due to using internet addiction scales to assess IGD (Billieux et al. 2015; Choi et al. 2014; Ding et al. 2014; Du et al. 2016; Du et al. 2017; Hyun et al. 2015; Kim et al. 2017a; Kim et al. 2017b; Lee et al. 2018; Lee et al. 2017; Rho et al. 2017; Ryu et al. 2018; Tian et al. 2018; Wang et al. 2016,2017; Wu et al. 2018; Yao et al. 2017), adopting modified tools based on anxiety related with general computing (Blinka et al. 2016; Gentile et al. 2011; Metcalf and Pammer 2014), internet addiction (Hu et al. 2017; Sariyska et al. 2017), or non-substance addiction criteria (Bailey et al. 2013; Liau et al. 2015; Rehbein et al. 2010; Walther et al. 2012).

Methodological Features of Studies

In regard to key methodological features of the studies, all studies were quantitative in nature. The majority of the studies (n = 25) employed a quantitative cross-sectional design (Bailey et al. 2013; Bargeron and Hormes 2017; Billieux et al. 2015; Blinka et al. 2016; Choi et al. 2014; Deleuze et al. 2017; Ding et al. 2014; Du et al. 2016; Du et al. 2017; Irvine et al. 2013; Kim et al. 2017a; Kim et al. 2017b; Lee et al. 2017; Lee et al. 2018; Metcalf and Pammer 2014; Nuyens et al. 2016; Rehbein et al. 2010; Rho et al. 2017; Sariyska et al. 2017; Stockdale and Coyne 2018; Tian et al. 2018; Walther et al. 2012; Wang et al. 2016,2017; Wu et al. 2018) and six studies adopted a correlational mixed-methods cross-sectional design (Hu et al. 2017; Hyun et al. 2015; Irles and Gomis 2016; Liau et al. 2015; Ryu et al. 2018; Yen et al. 2017). Additionally, two studies employed a longitudinal design (Gentile et al. 2011; Yao et al. 2017). Excluding 12 studies that combined both self-report questionnaires and clinical interviews (Choi et al. 2014; Ding et al. 2014; Du et al. 2017; Hyun et al. 2015; Irvine et al. 2013; Kim et al. 2017a; Kim et al. 2017b; Lee et al. 2017; Ryu et al. 2018; Tian et al. 2018; Wu et al. 2018; Yao et al. 2017) and one study which included only a clinical interview based on DSM-5 IGD Criteria — 9 Questions (Yen et al. 2017), the remaining 20 studies used self-report surveys to collect data. Moreover, while five studies employed online surveys with self-report measures for assessing IGD (Bargeron and Hormes 2017; Billieux et al. 2015; Blinka et al. 2016; Rho et al. 2017; Stockdale and Coyne 2018), 28 studies used offline “paper-and-pencil” survey methods (Bailey et al. 2013; Choi et al. 2014; Deleuze et al. 2017; Ding et al. 2014; Du et al. 2016; Du et al. 2017; Gentile et al. 2011; Hu et al. 2017; Hyun et al. 2015; Irles and Gomis 2016; Irvine et al. 2013; Kim et al. 2017a; Kim et al. 2017b; Lee et al. 2017; Lee et al. 2018; Liau et al. 2015; Metcalf and Pammer 2014; Nuyens et al. 2016; Rehbein et al. 2010; Ryu et al. 2018; Sariyska et al. 2017; Tian et al. 2018; Walther et al. 2012; Wang et al. 2016,2017; Wu et al. 2018; Yao et al. 2017; Yen et al. 2017). As to the sampling methods adopted, all the studies used convenience sampling or self-selected sampling to recruit participants. Several types of limitations were determined across all 33 studies (see Table 2) and can be grouped into three types: (a) sampling problems; (b) measurement problems; and (c) lack of longitudinal studies (i.e., excluding two studies, all of the remaining studies were cross-sectional). Sampling problems included (i) employing non-probability sampling methods (n = 24); (ii) sampling homogeneity due to including male-only samples within some studies (n = 10); (iii) the omission of clinical samples within the majority of studies (n = 24); and (iv) low sample sizes (n = 15 — five outpatient studies had samples sizes of 83 or fewer, and 10 other studies had sample sizes of 97 or fewer). Measurement problems included employing modified measurements that originally designed for evaluating generalized internet addiction and not validated by previous studies.

Discussion

The present systematic review identified peer-reviewed published studies investigating the association between impulsivity and Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD). The present study also extracted data from the reviewed studies including: (i) country in which data were collected, (ii) main characteristics of participants, (iii) operationalization of IGD, (iv) the association between impulsivity and IGD, (v) risk of bias in individual researches, and (vi) methodological features of the studies.

In terms of geographic location, more than half of the studies (n = 20) were conducted in an Asian context, with the others being conducted either in European countries and/or the USA. As for the participants involved in the studies reviewed, similar patterns appeared across the studies. More specifically, studies were prone to comprise: (i) more male participants than female, (ii) adult samples rather than adolescent samples, (iii) student samples rather than clinical samples and/or individuals from the general population, and (iv) non-MMORPG players rather than MMORPG players. Based on these observations, future studies investigating the association between impulsivity and IGD should include other participants (children and adolescents, females, clinical samples, and players from a variety of gaming genres) to enhance external validity.

With regard to the operationalization of IGD, while about a quarter reviewed studies used gaming addiction criteria, the others employed a modified measurement based on either internet addiction criteria, substance-related addiction criteria, or non-substance–related addiction criteria (e.g., pathological gambling criteria). Adopting such scales severely limits the reliability of the cross-cultural studies (Pontes and Griffiths 2014). In future studies, validated instruments specifically designed to assess IGD should be adopted and tested in different settings to improve cross-cultural reliability (Griffiths et al. 2014).

The main objective of the present review was to identify studies evaluating the association between impulsivity and IGD. Results demonstrated that apart from one study (Delueze et al. 2017), all of the remaining studies reported a positive association between impulsivity and IGD. Possible explanations for this consistent finding appear to indicate that altered neurobiological structures detected in participants with impulsivity may explain some of the relationship between impulsivity and internet gaming disorder. It is believed that impulsivity occurs as a result of neuropsychological impairments in attention and executive functions. In addition, although the causal relationships are not clearly defined, it has been argued that the clinical features of IGD may be related to structural abnormalities in the brain (Park et al. 2016). Some studies addressing gaming addiction have identified structural abnormalities within brain regions known to be associated with executive control such as anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), supplementary motor area (SMA), prefrontal and parietal cortices (Bush et al. 2000; Nachev et al. 2008; Crowe et al. 2013), and therefore IGD has been implicated with impairments in executive control (Du et al. 2016; Wang et al. 2016). Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) studies have shown that, in addition to the ACC, lower gray matter volume within dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) — which are well known to be associated with executive control — are related with IGD (Yuan et al. 2011). Furthermore, detecting a negative correlation between impulsivity and gray matter volume in the ACC and SMA has suggested that high impulsivity among gaming-addicted adolescents may be derived from structural abnormalities in brain regions involved in executive control (Lee et al. 2017).

More specifically, it has been reported that the most affected brain region is the ACC in IGD participants (Lee et al. 2017). As aforementioned, this region, which is involved in executive control, is particularly effective in the conflict monitoring (Botvinick et al. 2001) and error processing (Kiehl et al. 2000). The capacity for conflict and error monitoring is closely implicated with impulse control and delay of gratification (Checa et al. 2014). In parallel, Lee et al. (2014) showed that low-level gray matter volume in the ACC was associated with impulsivity among gaming addicts. Furthermore, the role of the ACC is emphasized in associating executive function with reward-based decision-making (Holroyd and Yeung 2012; Shenhav et al. 2013). In other words, the changes/differences in/of the ACC detected among gaming addicts may play an important role in the dysfunctional relationship between executive control and reward-seeking. This appears to support the I-PACE model of IGD (Brand et al. 2016). In addition, gaming addicts have lower gray matter volume within the SMA compared to control groups (Lee et al. 2014). It is known that abnormalities in the SMA, which has an important role in linking cognition to behavior (Nachev et al. 2008), cause response prevention deficits (Picton et al. 2007).

In parallel, a functional imaging study showed lower SMA activation in participants with IGD compared to controls in the response prevention task (Chen et al. 2015). DLPFC is a brain area involved in behavioral inhibition, reward processing, and decision-making (Hampshire et al. 2010; Menon and Uddin 2010; Staudinger et al. 2011). In particular, impaired functional connectivity in the bilateral prefrontal lobe has been found among IGD participants (Wang et al. 2015). IGD participants have been shown to exhibit lower activation in the left DLPFC compared to healthy groups while selecting the delayed option (Wang et al. 2017). Hoffman et al. (2008) showed that methamphetamine addicts had lower activation in DLPFC in delayed decisions compared with a healthy group. The dual-system mode posits that the DLPFC is involved in weighting/evaluating delayed rewards (McClure et al. 2004; McClure et al. 2007). It has also been shown that the DLPFC is responsible for postponing delayed rewards, and that activation in the DLPFC is negatively associated with increased delay time (Ballard and Knutson 2009). In other words, decreased brain activities in the DLPFC detected among gaming addicts may signify that they have potential impairments in assessing the magnitude and postponement of rewards (Wang et al. 2017).

Even if decision-making times increase, all of the information concerning options may not be unified/integrated, and therefore, their decision-making capacity may be negatively affected (Wang et al. 2017). Furthermore, it is thought that reduced functional connectivity strength between the DLPFC and the caudate among gaming addicts indicates that the DLPFC’s effective modulation of rewards has been disrupted (Yuan et al. 2017). A positive relationship between response time and severity of gaming addiction has been reported (Wang et al. 2017). This means that as the severity of gaming addiction increases, the time required to make choices increases. This relationship supports the assumption of impaired assessment skills of gaming addicts on the delayed features to a certain extent (Wang et al. 2017).

Taken together, it is argued that unconscious short-term gains in IGD are experienced and that this may be related to weak reward evaluation skills (Wang et al. 2017). Because of diminished activity in the brain regions associated with working memory and executive control, gaming addicts show decreased ability to inhibit their impulses for short-term gains (Wang et al. 2016). In addition to the known role in reward processing, the DLPFC is responsible for executive functions such as multi-attribute/qualified decision-making and response inhibition (Steinbeis et al. 2012; Zysset et al. 2006). More specifically, studies have shown that DLPFC activations increase when individuals experience self-control (Hare et al. 2009). In addition, decreased activation of the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) among gaming addicts has been determined during the inhibition processing (Wang et al. 2017).

The IFG is an effective region for cognitive control and impulse inhibition (Brown et al. 2012; Tops and Boksem 2011). The IFG is also responsible for the prevention of strong responses needed to abandon immediate gratification, self-control, seeking long-term gains (Aron et al. 2004; Garavan et al. 2002; Menon et al. 2001), and organizing (and establishing) a flexible relationship between outcomes and advantageous behaviors (actions) (Ernst and Paulus 2005). From this point of view, it is thought that the low brain activity within the DLPFC and IFG among gaming addicts indicates an impaired ability to control their behaviors and impulses (Wang et al. 2017). Many studies concerning IGD have reported diminished capacity for responding to immediate rewards and altered brain activities in the DLPFC (Liu et al.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric  Publishing.

  • Aron, A. R., Monsell, S., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2004). A componential analysis of task-switching deficits associated with lesions of left and right frontal cortex. Brain, 127(7), 1561–1573.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, K., West, R., & Kuffel, J. (2013). What would my avatar do? Gaming, pathology, and risky decision making. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 609.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ballard, K., & Knutson, B. (2009). Dissociable neural representations of future reward magnitude and delay during temporal discounting. Neuroimage, 45(1), 143–150.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bargeron, A. H., & Hormes, J. M. (2017). Psychosocial correlates of internet gaming disorder: Psychopathology, life satisfaction, and impulsivity. Computers in Human Behavior, 68, 388–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 1458–1463.

  • Billieux, J., Thorens, G., Khazaal, Y., Zullino, D., Achab, S., & Van der Linden, M. (2015). Problematic involvement in online games: A cluster analytic approach. Computers in Human Behavior, 43, 242–250.

  • Blinka, L., Škařupová, K., & Mitterova, K. (2016). Dysfunctional impulsivity in online gaming addiction and engagement. Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace, 10(3), 5. https://doi.org/10.5817/CP2016-3-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. (2001). Evaluating the demand for control: Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict monitoring. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.

  • Brand, M., Young, K. S., Laier, C., Wölfling, K., & Potenza, M. N. (2016). Integrating psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific internet–use disorders: An interaction of person–affect–cognition–execution (I–PACE) model. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 71, 252–266.

  • Brown, M. R., Lebel, R. M., Dolcos, F., Wilman, A. H., Silverstone, P. H., Pazderka, H., Fujiwara, E., Wild, T. C., Carroll, A. M., Hodlevskyy, O., Zedkova, L., Zwaigenbaum, L., Thompson, A. H., Greenshaw, A. J., & Dursun, S. M. (2012). Effects of emotional context on impulse control. Neuroimage, 63(1), 434–446.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 215–222.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cao, F., Su, L., Liu, T., & Gao, X. (2007). The relationship between impulsivity and internet addiction in a sample of Chinese adolescents. European Psychiatry, 22, 466–471.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Checa, P., Castellanos, M., Abundis-Gutiérrez, A., & Rueda, M. R. (2014). Development of neural mechanisms of conflict and error processing during childhood: implications for self-regulation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 326.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, C. Y., Huang, M. F., Yen, J. Y., Chen, C. S., Liu, G. C., Yen, C. F., & Ko, C. H. (2015). Brain correlates of response inhibition in internet gaming disorder. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 69, 201–209.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, B., Lee, I., Choi, D., & Kim, J. (2007). Collaborate and share: an experimental study of the effects of task and reward interdependencies in online games. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 10, 591–595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Choi, S. W., Kim, H., Kim, G. Y., Jeon, Y., Park, S., Lee, J. Y., Jung, H., Sohn, B., Choi, J. S., & Kim, D. J. (2014). Similarities and differences among internet gaming disorder, gambling disorder and alcohol use disorder: A focus on impulsivity and compulsivity. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 3, 246–253.

  • Crowe, D. A., Goodwin, S. J., Blackman, R. K., Sakellaridi, S., Sponheim, S. R., MacDonald, A. W., & Chafee, M. V. (2013). Prefrontal neurons transmit signals to parietal neurons that reflect executive control of cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16, 1484–1491.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow, the psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cyders, M. A., & Smith, G. T. (2008). Emotion-based dispositions to rash action: Positive and negative urgency. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 807–828.

  • D’Zurilla, T. J., Chang, E. C., & Sanna, L. J. (2003). Self-esteem and social problem solving as predictors of aggression in college students. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22(4), 424–440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dantzker, M. L., & Hunter, R. (2011). Research methods for criminology and criminal justice (Third ed.). London: Jones & Bartett Learning.

  • Deleuze, J., Nuyens, F., Rochat, L., Rothen, S., Maurage, P., & Billieux, J. (2017). Established risk factors for addiction fail to discriminate between healthy gamers and gamers endorsing DSM-5 internet gaming disorder. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(4), 516–524.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ding, W. N., Sun, J. H., Sun, Y. W., Chen, X., Zhou, Y., Zhuang, Z. G., Li, L., Zhang, Y., Xu, J. R., & Du, Y. S. (2014). Trait impulsivity and impaired prefrontal impulse inhibition function in adolescents with internet gaming addiction revealed by a go/no-go fMRI study. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 10(1), 20.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dong, G., Huang, J., & Du, X. (2011). Enhanced reward sensitivity and decreased loss sensitivity in internet addicts: An fMRI study during a guessing task. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45, 1525–1529.

  • Dong, G., Devito, E. E., Du, X., & Cui, Z. (2012). Impaired inhibitory control in ‘internet addiction disorder’s functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychiatry Research, 203, 153–158.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dong, G., Shen, Y., Huang, J., & Du, X. (2013). Impaired error-monitoring function in people with internet addiction disorder: An event-related fMRI study. European Addiction Research, 19, 269–275.

  • Dong, G., Lin, X., Zhou, H., & Lu, Q. (2014). Cognitive flexibility in internet addicts: fMRI evidence from difficult-to-easy and easy-to- difficult switching situations. Addictive Behaviors, 39, 677–683.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dowling, N. A. (2014). Issues raised by the DSM-5 internet gaming disorder classification and proposed diagnostic criteria. Addiction, 109(9), 1408–1409.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Du, X., Qi, X., Yang, Y., Du, G., Gao, P., Zhang, Y., Qin, W., Li, X., & Zhang, Q. (2016). Altered structural correlates of impulsivity in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 4.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Du, X., Liu, L., Yang, Y., Qi, X., Gao, P., Zhang, Y., Zhu, J., Du, G., Dai, S., Li, X., & Zhang, Q. (2017). Diffusion tensor imaging of the structural integrity of white matter correlates with impulsivity in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. Brain and Behavior: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective, 7(8), 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ernst, M., & Paulus, M. P. (2005). Neurobiology of decision making: A selective review from a neurocognitive and clinical perspective. Biological Psychiatry, 58(8), 597–604.

  • Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., Murphy, K., Roche, R. A. P., & Stein, E. A. (2002). Dissociable executive functions in the dynamic control of behavior: Inhibition, error detection, and correction. Neuroimage, 17(4), 1820–1829.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentile, D. A., Choo, H., Liau, A., Sim, T., Li, D., Fung, D., & Khoo, A. (2011). Pathological video game use among youths: A two-year longitudinal study. Pediatrics, 127(2), e319–e329.

  • Goldstein, R. Z., & Volkow, N. D. (2011). Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: Neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 652–669.

  • Granö, N., Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L., Kouvonen, A., Virtanen, M., Elovainio, M., Vahtera, J., & Kivimäki, M. (2007). Impulsivity as a predictor of newly diagnosed depression. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 48, 173–179.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, M. (2003). Internet gambling: Issues, concerns and recommendations. CyberPsychology and Behavior, 6(6), 557–568.

  • Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., & King, D. L. (2012). Video game addiction: Past, present, and future. Current Psychiatry Reviews, 8, 308–318.

  • Griffiths, M., King, D., & Demetrovics, Z. (2014). DSM-5 internet gaming disorder needs a unified approach to assessment. Neuropsychiatry, 4(1), 1–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, M. D., van Rooij, A. J., Kardefelt-Winther, D., Starcevic, V., Király, O., … Demetrovics, Z. (2016). Working towards an international consensus on criteria for assessing Internet gaming disorder: A critical commentary on Petry et al. (2014). Addiction, 111(1), 167–175.

  • Gunuc, S. (2015). Relationships and associations between video game and internet addictions: Is tolerance a symptom seen in all conditions. Computers in Human Behavior, 49, 517–525.

  • Hampshire, A., Chamberlain, S. R., Monti, M. M., Duncan, J., & Owen, A. M. (2010). The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control. Neuroimage, 50(3), 1313–1319.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Han, D. H., Kim, Y. S., Lee, Y. S., Min, K. J., & Renshaw, P. F. (2010). Changes in cue-induced, prefrontal cortex activity with video-game play. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 13, 655–661.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., & Rangel, A. (2009). Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science, 324(5927), 646–648.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, W. F., Schwartz, D. L., Huckans, M. S., McFarland, B. H., Meiri, G., Stevens, A. A., & Mitchell, S. H. (2008). Cortical activation during delay discounting in abstinent methamphetamine dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology, 201(2), 183–193.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Holroyd, C. B., & Yeung, N. (2012). Motivation of extended behaviors by anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16, 122–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hu, J., Zhen, S., Yu, C., Zhang, Q., & Zhang, W. (2017). Sensation seeking and online gaming addiction in adolescents: A moderated mediation model of positive affective associations and impulsivity. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 699.

  • Hyun, G. J., Han, D. H., Lee, Y. S., Kang, K. D., Yoo, S. K., Chung, U. S., & Renshaw, P. F. (2015). Risk factors associated with online game addiction: a hierarchical model. Computers in Human Behavior, 48, 706–713.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irles, D. L., & Gomis, R. M. (2016). Impulsiveness and video game addiction. Health and Addictions: Salud y Drogas, 16(1), 33–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irvine, M. A., Worbe, Y., Bolton, S., Harrison, N. A., Bullmore, E. T., & Voon, V. (2013). Impaired decisional impulsivity in pathological videogamers. PLoS One, 8(10), e75914.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kiehl, K. A., Liddle, P. F., & Hopfinger, J. B. (2000). Error processing and the rostral anterior cingulate: An event–related fMRI study. Psychophysiology, 37, 216–223.

  • Kim, M., Lee, T. H., Choi, J. S., Kwak, Y. B., Hwang, W. J., Kim, T., Lee, J. Y., Lim, J., Park, M., Kim, Y. J., Kim, S. N., Kim, D. J., & Kwon, J. S. (2017a). Neurophysiological correlates of altered response inhibition in internet gaming disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: Perspectives from impulsivity and compulsivity. Scientific Reports, 7, 41742.

  • Kim, Y. J., Lim, J. A., Lee, J. Y., Oh, S., Kim, S. N., Kim, D. J., Ha, J. E., Kwon, J. S., & Choi, J. S. (2017b). Impulsivity and compulsivity in internet gaming disorder: A comparison with obsessive–compulsive disorder and alcohol use disorder. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(4), 545–553.

  • Király, O., & Demetrovics, Z. (2017). Inclusion of gaming disorder in ICD has more advantages than disadvantages: Commentary on: scholars’ open debate paper on the World Health Organization ICD-11 gaming disorder proposal (Aarseth et al.). Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(3), 1–15.

  • Ko, C. H., Hsieh, T. J., Wang, P. W., Lin, W. C., Yen, C. F., Chen, C. S., & Yen, J. Y. (2015). Altered gray matter density and disrupted functional connectivity of the amygdala in adults with internet gaming disorder. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 57, 185–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koepp, M. J., Gunn, R. N., Lawrence, A. D., Cunningham, V. J., Dagher, A., Jones, T., Brooks, D. J., Bench, C. J., & Grasby, P. M. (1998). Evidence for striatal dopamine release during a video game. Nature, 393, 266–268.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2012). Internet gaming addiction: A systematic review of empirical research. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 10, 278–296.

  • Lee, Y. S., Son, J. H., Park, J. H., Kim, S. M., Kee, B. S., & Han, D. H. (2017). The comparison of temperament and character between patients with internet gaming disorder and those with alcohol dependence. Journal of Mental Health, 26(3), 242–247.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D., Namkoong, K., Lee, J., & Jung, Y. C. (2018). Abnormal gray matter volume and impulsivity in young adults with internet gaming disorder. Addiction Biology, 23(5), 1160–1167.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liau, A. K., Neo, E. C., Gentile, D. A., Choo, H., Sim, T., Li, D., & Khoo, A. (2015). Impulsivity, self-regulation, and pathological video gaming among youth: Testing a mediation model. Asia-Pacific Journal of Public Health, 27(2), NP2188–NP2196.

  • Lin, X., Zhou, H., Dong, G., & Du, X. (2015). Impaired risk evaluation in people with internet gaming disorder: fMRI evidence from a probability discounting task. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 56, 142–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, L., Xue, G., Potenza, M. N., Zhang, J. T., Yao, Y. W., **a, C. C., Lan, J., Ma, S. S., & Fang, X. Y. (2017). Dissociable neural processes during risky decision-making in individuals with internet-gaming disorder. Neuroimage Clinical, 14, 741–749.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mai, Y., Hu, J., Yan, Z., Zhen, S., Wang, S., & Zhang, W. (2012). Structure and function of maladaptive cognitions in pathological internet use among Chinese adolescents. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 2376–2386.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306(5695), 503–507.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClure, S. M., Ericson, K. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2007). Time discounting for primary rewards. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(21), 5796–5804.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mehroof, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2010). Online gaming addiction: The role of sensation seeking, self-control, neuroticism, aggression, state anxiety, and trait anxiety. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 13, 313–316.

  • Menon, V., & Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Structure and Function, 214(5–6), 655–667.

  • Menon, V., Adleman, N. E., White, C. D., Glover, G. H., & Reiss, A. L. (2001). Error-related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task. Human Brain Map**, 12(3), 131–143.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalf, O., & Pammer, K. (2014). Impulsivity and related neuropsychological features in regular and addictive first person shooter gaming. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 17(3), 147–152.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nachev, P., Kennard, C., & Husain, M. (2008). Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 856–869.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nuyens, F., Deleuze, J., Maurage, P., Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., & Billieux, J. (2016). Impulsivity in multiplayer online battle arena gamers: Preliminary results on experimental and self-report measures. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5(2), 351–356.

  • Park, B., Han, D. H., & Roh, S. (2016). Neurobiological findings related to internet use disorders. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 71(7), 467–478.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Picton, T. W., Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Shallice, T., Binns, M. A., & Gillingham, S. (2007). Effects of focal frontal lesions on response inhibition. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 826–838.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pontes, H. M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2014). Assessment of internet gaming disorder in research: past and present perspectives. Clinical Research and Regulatory Affairs, 31(24), 35–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pontes, H. M., Kiraly, O., Demetrovics, Z., & Griffiths, M. D. (2014). The conceptualisation and measurement of DSM-5 internet gaming disorder: The development of the IGD–20 test. PLoS One, 9(10), e110137.

  • Rehbein, F., Psych, G., Kleimann, M., Mediasci, G., & Mößle, T. (2010). Prevalence and risk factors of video game dependency in adolescence: Results of a German nationwide survey. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 13(3), 269–277.

  • Rho, M. J., Lee, H., Lee, T. H., Cho, H., Jung, D. J., Kim, D. J., & Choi, I. Y. (2017). Risk factors for internet gaming disorder: psychological factors and internet gaming characteristics. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(1), 40.

    PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., Rigby, C. S., & Przybylski, A. (2006). The motivational pull of video games: A self-determination theory approach. Motivation and Emotion, 30(4), 344–360.

  • Ryu, H., Lee, J. Y., Choi, A., Park, S., Kim, D. J., & Choi, J. S. (2018). The relationship between impulsivity and internet gaming disorder in young adults: mediating effects of interpersonal relationships and depression. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(3), 458.

    PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sariyska, R., Lachmann, B., Markett, S., Reuter, M., & Montag, C. (2017). Individual differences in implicit learning abilities and impulsive behavior in the context of internet addiction and internet gaming disorder under the consideration of gender. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 5, 19–28.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Savci, M., & Aysan, F. (2016). Relationship between impulsivity, social media usage and loneliness. Educational Process International Journal, 5, 106–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron, 79, 217–240.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Staudinger, M. R., Erk, S., & Walter, H. (2011). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex modulates striatal reward encoding during reappraisal of reward anticipation. Cerebral Cortex, 21(11), 2578–2588.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steinbeis, N., Bernhardt, B., & Singer, T. (2012). Impulse control and underlying functions of the left DLPFC mediate age-related and age-independent individual differences in strategic social behavior. Neuron, 73(5), 1040–1051.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauman, E., Banich, M., Graham, S., & Woolard, J. (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: evidence for a dual systems model. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1764–1778.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, M. E., Ebmeier, K. P., & Deary, I. J. (2004). The structure of Cloninger’s Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire in a British sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(6), 1403–1418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stockdale, L., & Coyne, S. M. (2018). Video game addiction in emerging adulthood: Cross-sectional evidence of pathology in video game addicts as compared to matched healthy controls. Journal of Affective Disorders, 225, 265–272.

  • Swann, A. C., Steinberg, J. L., Lijffijt, M., & Moeller, F. G. (2008). Impulsivity: Differential relationship to depression and mania in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 106, 241–248.

  • Tian, M., Tao, R., Zheng, Y., Zhang, H., Yang, G., Li, Q., & Liu, X. (2018). Internet gaming disorder in adolescents is linked to delay discounting but not probability discounting. Computers in Human Behavior, 80, 59–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tops, M., & Boksem, M. A. S. (2011). A potential role of the inferior frontal gyrus and anterior insula in cognitive control, brain rhythms, and event-related potentials. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 330.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • van Holst, R. J., Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J., Veltman, D. J., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2012). Attentional bias and disinhibition toward gaming cues are related to problem gaming in male adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 50(6), 541–546.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walther, B., Morgenstern, M., & Hanewinkel, R. (2012). Co–occurrence of addictive behaviours: Personality factors related to substance use, gambling and computer gaming. European Addiction Research, 18(4), 167–174.

  • Wang, Y., Yin, Y., Sun, Y. W., Zhou, Y., Chen, X., Ding, W. N., Wang, W., Li, W., Xu, J. R., & Du, Y. S. (2015). Decreased prefrontal lobe interhemispheric functional connectivity in adolescents with internet gaming disorder: A primary study using resting-state FMRI. PLoS One, 10(3), e0118733.

  • Wang, Y., Wu, L., Wang, L., Zhang, Y., Du, X., & Dong, G. (2016b). Impaired decision-making and impulse control in internet gaming addicts: evidence from the comparison with recreational internet game users. Addiction Biology, 22(6), 1610–1621.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Y., Hu, Y., Xu, J., Zhou, H., Lin, X., Du, X., & Dong, G. (2017). Dysfunctional prefrontal function is associated with impulsivity in people with internet gaming disorder during a delay discounting task. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8, 287.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, Y., Sescousse, G., Yu, H., Clark, L., & Li, H. (2018). Cognitive distortions and gambling near misses in internet gaming disorder: A preliminary study. PLoS One, 13(1), e0191110.

  • Yao, Y. W., Chen, P. R., Chiang-Shan, R. L., Hare, T. A., Li, S., Zhang, J. T., Liu, L., Ma, S. S., & Fang, X. Y. (2017). Combined reality therapy and mindfulness meditation decrease intertemporal decisional impulsivity in young adults with internet gaming disorder. Computers in Human Behavior, 68, 210–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yee, N. (2006). Motivations for play in online games. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 9, 772–775.

  • Yen, J. Y., Liu, T. L., Wang, P. W., Chen, C. S., Yen, C. F., & Ko, C. H. (2017). Association between internet gaming disorder and adult attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder and their correlates: impulsivity and hostility. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 308–313.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yuan, K., Qin, W., Wang, G., Zeng, F., Zhao, L., Yang, X., Liu, P., Liu, J., Sun, J., & von Deneen, K. M. (2011). Microstructure abnormalities in adolescents with internet addiction disorder. PLoS One, 6, e20708.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zysset, S., Wendt, C. S., Volz, K. G., Neumann, J., Huber, O., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2006). The neural implementation of multi-attribute decision making: A parametric fMRI study with human subjects. Neuroimage, 31(3), 1380–1388.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mark D. Griffiths.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

Not applicable.

Informed Consent

Not applicable.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Şalvarlı, Ş.İ., Griffiths, M.D. The Association Between Internet Gaming Disorder and Impulsivity: A Systematic Review of Literature. Int J Ment Health Addiction 20, 92–118 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-019-00126-w

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-019-00126-w

Keywords