Log in

An Eye-Tracking Examination of Emotion Regulation, Attentional Bias, and Pupillary Response to Threat Stimuli

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In the present study, we examined constructs of emotion regulation (emotion regulation difficulties, expressive suppression, and cognitive reappraisal) in relation to attentional bias to threat (ABT). Participants (N = 176) completed a battery of self-report measures and an eye-tracking task in which eye movements to neutral and threat images were recorded. Dwell time on threat was examined across six 500 ms intervals for each trial. We also examined pupillary response as a measure of emotional arousal. When accounting for cognitive reappraisal and emotion regulation difficulties, expressive suppression significantly predicted both ABT (from 1500–3000 ms) and pupillary response to threat. However, the effect of expression suppression on ABT was significant only at lower levels of cognitive reappraisal. Emotion regulation difficulties did not significantly predict ABT or pupillary response. Findings suggest that those who use expressive suppression to the exclusion of other regulatory strategies may be at particularly high risk for ABT and relatively higher levels of threat-related emotional arousal. Clinical implications, as well as results of an exploratory analysis examining dimensions of emotion regulation difficulties as they relate to ABT and pupillary response, will be discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price includes VAT (France)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. To increase confidence that the observed associations between emotion regulation-related constructs and ABT and pupillary reactivity were not a function of the relations of these variables with emotional distress, or affect more generally, we repeated our primary analyses and included measures of positive and negative affect as covariates (i.e., Positive and Negative Affect Schedule: Watson et al. 1988). Results were consistent with our initial analysis; statistically significant findings remained significant and nonsignificant findings were unchanged.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allison, P. D. (2002). Missing data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Amir, I., Zvielli, A., & Bernstein, A. (2016). (De)coupling of our eye and our minds eye: A dynamic process perspective on attentional bias. Emotion, 16, 978–986.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, T., Blisky, S. A., Zhao, M., & Olatunji, B. O. (2013). Dwelling on potential threat cues: An eye movement marker for combat-related PTSD. Depression and Anxiety, 30, 497–502.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, T., Olatunji, B. O., Sarawgi, S., & Simmons, C. (2010). Orienting and maintenance of gaze in contamination fear: biases for disgust and fear cues. Behavior Research and Therapy, 48, 402–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bardeen, J. R. (2015). Short-term pain for long-term gain: The role of experiential avoidance in the relation between anxiety sensitivity and emotional distress. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 30, 113–119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bardeen, J. R., Daniel, T. A., Hinnant, J. B., & Orcutt, H. K. (2017). Emotion dysregulation and threat-related attention bias variability. Motivation & Emotion, 41, 402–409.

  • Bardeen, J. R., Fergus, T. A., Hannan, S. M., & Orcutt, H. K. (2016). Addressing psychometric limitations of the difficulties in emotion regulation Scale through item modification. Journal of Personality Assessment, 98, 298–309.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bardeen, J. R., Fergus, T. A., & Orcutt, H. K. (2012). An examination of the latent structure of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 34, 382–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bardeen, J. R., Tull, M. T., Daniel, T. A., Evenden, J., & Stevens, E. N. (2016). A preliminary investigation of the time course of attention bias variability in posttraumatic stress disorder: The moderating role of attentional control. Behaviour Change, 33, 94–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Millar, N., Bonham-Carter, C., Fergusson, E., … Parr, M. (1997) Attentional biases for emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 25–42

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., Miccoli, L., Escrig, M. A., & Lang, P. J. (2008). The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology, 45, 602–607.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner, J. D., Maner, J. K., & Schmidt, N. B. (2010). Difficulty disengaging attention from social threat in social anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 34, 99–105.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Catanzaro, S. J., & Greenwood, G. (1994). Expectancies for negative mood regulation, co**, and dysphoria among college students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 41, 34–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chesney, S. A., & Gordon, N. S. (2017). Profiles of emotion regulation: Understanding regulatory patterns and the implications for posttraumatic stress. Cognition and Emotion, 31, 598–606.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cisler, J. M., & Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in in anxiety disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 203–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. A. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd edn.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Compton, J. R. (2000). Ability to disengage attention predicts negative affect. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 401–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denny, B. T., Inhoff, M. C., Zerubavel, N., Davachi, L., & Ochsner, K. N. (2015). Getting over it: Long-lasting effects of emotion regulation on amygdala response. Psychological Science, 26, 1377–1388.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon-Gordon, K. L., Aldao, A., & De Los Reyes, A. (2014). Repertoires of emotion regulation: A person-centered approach to assessing emotion regulation strategies and links to psychopathology. Cognition and Emotion, 29, 1314–1325.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duque, A., Sanchez, A., & Vazquez, C. (2014). Gaze-fixation and pupil dilation in the processing of emotional faces: The role of rumination. Cognition and Emotion, 28, 1347–1366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ehring, T., & Quack, D. (2010). Emotion regulation difficulties in trauma survivors: The role of trauma type and PTSD symptom severity. Behavior Therapy, 41, 587–598.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellenbogen, M. A., Schwartzman, A. E., Stewart, J., & Walker, C. D. (2006). Automatic and effortful emotional information processing regulates different aspects of the stress response. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31, 373–387.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied Missing Data Analysis. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erk, S., Kiefer, M., Grothe, J., Wunderlich, A. P., Spitzer, M., & Walter, H. (2003). Emotional context modulates subsequent memory effect. NeuroImage, 18, 439–447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Granholm, E., Asarnow, R. F., Sarkin, A. J., & Dykes, K. L. (1996). Pupillary responses index cognitive resource limitations. Psychophysiology, 33, 457–461.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26, 41–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348–362.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiology, self–report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 970–986.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting positive and negative emotions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 95–103.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holmqvist, K., Nyström, N., Andersson, R., Dewhurst, R., Jarodzka, H., & Weijer, J. Van de (2011) In: Holmqvist, K., Nyström, N., Andersson, R., Dewhurst, R., Jarodzka, H., & Weijer, J. Van de (Eds.) Eye tracking: a comprehensive guide to methods and measures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoorelbeke, K., Marchetti, I., De Schryver, M., & Koster, E. H. W. (2016). The interplay between cognitive risk and resilience factors in remitted depression: A network analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 195, 96–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of Personality, 72, 1301–1334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lacey, J. I., & Lacey, B. C. (1980). The specific role of heart rate in sensorimotor integration. In: R. F. Thompson, L. H. Hicks & V. B. Shvyrkov (Eds.), Neural mechanisms of goal-directed behavior. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1999). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical Manual and Affective Ratings. Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D. J., Witte, T. K., Bardeen, J. R., Davis, M. T., & Weathers, F. W. (2016). A factor analytic evaluation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 72, 933–946.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, H. J., & Telch, M. J. (2008). Attentional biases in social anxiety: An investigation using the inattentional blindness paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 819–835.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levy-Gigi, E., Bonanno, G. A., Shapiro, A. R., Richter-Levin, G., Keri, S., & Sheppes, G. (2016). Emotion regulatory flexibility sheds light on the elusive relationship between repeated traumatic exposure and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms. Clinical Psychological Science, 4, 28–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manera, V., Samson, A. C., Pehrs, C., Lee, I. A., & Gross, J. J. (2014). The eyes have it: The role of attention in cognitive reappraisal of social stimuli. Emotion, 14, 833–839.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McNally, R. J., Riemann, B. C., & Kim, E. (1990). Selective processing of threat cues in panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 407–412.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nixon, R. D., & Rackebrandt, J. (2016). Cognitive load undermines thought suppression in acute stress disorder. Behavior Therapy, 47, 388–403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Partala, T., & Surakka, V. (2003). Pupil size variation as an indication of affective processing. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 59, 185–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pineles, S. L., Shipherd, J. C., Mostoufi, S. M., Abramovitz, S. M., & Yovel, I. (2009). Attentional biases in PTSD: More evidence for interference. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 1050–1057.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pretz, J. E., Totz, K. S., & Kaufman, S. B. (2010). The effects of mood, cognitive style, and cognitive ability on implicit learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 3, 215–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanchez, A., Vazquez, C., Gomez, D., & Joormann, J. (2014). Gaze-fixation to happy faces predicts mood repair after a negative mood induction. Emotion, 14, 85–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanchez, A., Vazquez, C., Marker, C., LeMoult, J., & Joormann, J. (2013). Attentional disengagement predicts stress recovery in depression: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 303–313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmukle, S. C. (2005). Unreliability of the dot probe task. European Journal of Personality, 19, 595–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scult, M. A., Knodt, A. R., Swartz, J. R., Brigidi, B. D., & Hariri, A. R. (2017). Thinking and feeling: Inidividual differences in habitual emotion regulation and stress-related mood are associated with prefrontal executive control. Clinical Psychological Science, 5, 150–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seligowski, A. V., Lee, D. J., Bardeen, J. R., & Orcutt, H. K. (2015). Emotion Regulation and Posttraumatic Stress Symptoms: A Meta-Analysis. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 44, 87–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Emotion-regulation choice. Psychological Science, 11, 1391–1396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, G. P., Ossenfort, K. L., & Whearty, K. M. (2016). Reappraisal and distraction emotion regulation strategies are associated with distinct patterns of visual attention and differing levels of cognitive demand. PLoS ONE, 11(11), e0162290.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., Urry, H. L., Thurow, M. E., Schaefer, H. S., Alexander, A. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Gaze fixations predict brain activation during the voluntary regulation of picture-induced negative affect. NeuroImage, 36, 1041–1055.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vanderhasselt, M. A., Baeken, C., Van Schuerbeek, P., Luypaert, R., & De Raedt, R. (2013). Inter-individual differences in the habitual use of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression are associate with variations in prefrontal cognitive control for emotional information: An event related fMRI study. Biological Psychology, 92, 433–439.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, J., & De Houwer, J. (2014). Emotion regulation meets emotional attention: The influence of emotion suppression on emotional attention depends on the nature of the distracters. Emotion, 14, 840–845.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 59–91.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zvielli, A., Bernstein, A., & Koster, E. H. W. (2015). Temporal dynamics of attentional bias. Clinical Psychological Science, 5, 772–788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Erin Stevens for her valuable feedback and assistance in editing this manuscript and Natasha Benfer, Danielle DeLoach, Darian Crowley, Bailey Hall, Travis Rogers, and Tyler Toledo for assistance with data collection.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joseph R. Bardeen.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

Joseph R. Bardeen and Thomas A. Daniel declare that they have no conflict of interest. Additionally, the results of the submitted manuscript have not been presented elsewhere.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

This study was approved by the Office of Research Compliance at Auburn University. All participants provided informed consent prior to the completion of any study measures.

Animal Rights Statement

No animal studies were carried out by the authors for this article.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bardeen, J.R., Daniel, T.A. An Eye-Tracking Examination of Emotion Regulation, Attentional Bias, and Pupillary Response to Threat Stimuli. Cogn Ther Res 41, 853–866 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-017-9860-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-017-9860-y

Keywords

Navigation