Introduction

On March 11, 2020, the World Health Organisation (WHO) called on countries to take urgent and decisive action against the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) [1]. Governments around the world introduced strict public health measures to mitigate the spread of the disease, and while these measures have indeed been effective [2, 3], there are indications of deleterious mental health effects on children and adults [4,5,6,7]. It is particularly important to examine the effects on children, not only as research is scarce [8], but also as children are vulnerable to environmental changes and effects may have long-term consequences into adulthood [9]. We propose that effects on children should be studies more holistically by exploring a broader range of reactions rather than focusing on symptomatology of mental health difficulties. To this end, the present study focuses on a broader spectrum of children’s emotional reactions, along with children’s somatic and cognitive reactions, and worry reactions relating to parents and family. By exploring the normal range of reactions, we are better placed to understanding the breadth of implications of the pandemic on children.

Consequences of school closure and social isolation under COVID-19

The biggest change to children’s daily lives under COVID-19, might have been the closure of schools and introduction of digital home schooling. School is a place of academic learning, but also an arena for development, socialization and connecting with friends and peers, and for emotional and academic support from teachers, which are all important factors for children’s psychological wellbeing and adjustment [10,11,12,13]. School routines further allow children to have regular bed/wake times and physical activity, and restricts sedentary behaviours and/or non-educational screen time [14]. Conversely, school closures under COVID-19 has been associated with academic learning losses [15] and an array of health risk behaviours (e.g., socio-emotional complications, reduced physical activity) [16].

A marked difference between school closure under COVID-19 and more regular school closure (e.g., during holidays, teacher strikes), is that students continued their schoolwork digitally in the absence of physical contact. Children have varied in their experience with the new school day, in terms of among other things, their ability to concentrate on school work, meeting assignment deadlines, and their perceived level of support (or lack thereof) from teachers and parents. For some it may have been a blessing with increased independence, while for others, it would have been a struggle with motivation and self-discipline. And in particular, younger children seem to have done more poorly [17, 18].

Home schooling under COVID-19 was accompanied by strict social isolation measures and thus, children had involuntary restrictions placed on their opportunities to meet friends and peers physically although they may not have adhered (strictly) to these recommendations [19]. Evidence suggests that social isolation during the pandemic was associated with loneliness, negative consequences on mental health and other health-related behaviours for children [20, 21]. For example, two Chinese studies report elevated levels of symptoms of depression and anxiety in children and adolescents during the early phase of the pandemic. Children who worried about virus infection were significantly more likely to experience symptoms of depression, but not anxiety, compared to those that were not or only slightly worried about being infected [5, 31].

Consequences for children of different ages

It is noteworthy, that generally and under COVID-19, the impact of social isolation (and loneliness) on the mental health of children has been disproportionately explored in adolescent and older children [e.g., 5, 20, 23, 24]. Taking a child developmental perspective, the association between isolation and mental health should be stronger in adolescent and older children [32], given their heightened experience of emotional reactions, underdeveloped self-regulatory mechanisms and heightened motivation for peer affiliation and support. Developmentally, it is a time characterised by increasing independence from parents, increasing autonomy and peer friendships that become closer and more supportive [33]. Thus, there is a gradual shift in focus from the family to peers and friends [34]. The social isolation measures under COVID-19, may have been particularly challenging for older children as these have impeded close physical contact with friends and support from friends. The renegotiation of family rules by parents as mentioned earlier, may also have challenged older children’s developmental trajectory towards independence and autonomy. Therefore, to better understand the implications of home schooling and social isolation on children′s reactions, it is paramount to include a broader age-range of children and to explore the moderating effect of children’s age (as a proxy for their developmental stage) on any observed relationships.

The present study

Little is about how children have reacted to the new everyday life under COVID-19 with home schooling and social isolation. The available evidence from cross-sectional studies suggests that elevated levels of depression and anxiety during lockdown are related to variables such as worry about virus infection and social media use. Claims that other variables such as home school experiences and family functioning are related to how children react need a better empirical foundation. Longitudinal studies focusing on younger and older children’s reactions more broadly and accounting for any pre-existing psychological vulnerabilities better capture the dynamics of how children have reacted under the COVID-19 pandemic. To this end, we use data from a longitudinal study to explore the following questions:

  1. 1.

    How do children compare their reactions under COVID-19 with home schooling and social isolation to before the lockdown?

  2. 2.

    How are children’s reactions under COVID-19 associated with home school experience, perceived stress and instability in the family, screen time use, missing friends, and worry about virus infection?

  3. 3.

    Does children’s age moderate any of these associations?

Methods

Study design and participants

The data for this study were drawn from the Norwegian Family Dynamics Study (FamilieForSK), a longitudinal study aimed at increasing knowledge about family dynamics and conflicts in Norwegian families. The FamilieForSK-study has more than 2300 participating families, recruited through family counselling centres from December 2017 to July 2019 when families attended mandatory mediation (in relation to divorce/relationship dissolution), counselling or family therapy. Participating parents and children completed online questionnaires covering a wide range of topics, while trained interviewers interviewed younger children (7–11 years of age). In some families only one or both parents participated, while in other families the parent(s) and their child(ren) participated and finally, in a small number of families only the child(ren) participated. Shortly after the Norwegian government introduced public lockdown measures to mitigate the spread of COVID-19, FamilieForSK initiated an extraordinary data collection (Wave 3) to explore the experience of these measures on families. Parents and children that had already participated in Waves 1 and 2 were invited to participate in Wave 3, while parents and children due to participate in Wave 2 were invited to participate in Waves 2 and 3 at the same time (i.e., Wave 3 survey was joined with Wave 2 survey). The present study uses data from the children who participated at Wave 3 (April 1 to May 25, 2020), of whom nearly 85% also participated at Wave 1. There were no significant differences between children who only participated in Wave 1(n = 573) and children who participated in both Waves 1 and 3 (n = 374) in terms of symptoms of anxiety and depression and whether their parents lived together or lived apart. However, children who participated in Waves 1 and 3 were significantly younger than children who only participated in Wave 1 (M = 10.15 (SD = 2.53) vs. M = 10.91 (SD = 2.50), t(934) = 4.54, p < 0.001). See Table 1 for an overview of the sample characteristics.

Table 1 Sample descriptive statistics (N = 442)

In Norway, the Government closed all schools on March 12 and digital home schooling was introduced. Schools re-opened gradually from April 27 and younger children (up to grade 4) were the first to return to school. The school day was somewhat different to before the pandemic, as class sizes were reduced and children were grouped into smaller cohorts to reduce the possibility of spread of the disease [35]. The present study includes a small number of children who participated after the schools had opened, but note that only four of these children were younger and had potentially returned to school. Furthermore, the majority of children that participated after April 27 did so within just a few days. We decided nonetheless to include all children that had responded up until the point of May 25.

The Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics in Norway approved the study and all study procedures fulfilled the recommendations of the Helsinki Declaration. Parents consented for children to participate in the FamilieForSK- study and children assented before completing the online survey or before being interviewed by trained interviewers.

Measures

We assessed children’s reactions to the new everyday life with home schooling and social isolation with ten statements with the item stem “After the schools closed in March, I have …”. Items were developed specifically for the study. An initial Principal Component Analysis (PCA) with a promax rotation showed that items formed three components; Emotional Reactions with five items (e.g., “felt sad”, “felt angry”, “felt lonely”), Somatic/cognitive Reactions with three items (e.g., “had trouble concentrating”, “had headaches, stomach ache and so on”), and Worry Reactions with two items (e.g., “worried about my parents”). Children answered items on a scale from 0 (“A lot less than before”) to 4 (“A lot more than before”), with the middle value (2) representing “As before”. The Emotional and Somatic/cognitive reactions scales had acceptable internal reliability (α = 0.79 and α = 0.66, respectively) and the two worry items were moderately correlated (rpolychoric = 0.59, p < 0.05). See Tables 3 for descriptive statistics including item factor loadings.

Home school experience was assessed with four statements about how children managed home schooling, their concentration level, and home school support or lack thereof (reverse scored). Items were answered on a scale from 0 (“Not true”) to 2 (“Certainly true”) and internal reliability was acceptable (α = 0.72).

Family stress and instability was assessed with three statements tap** children’s perception of parent stress levels, and instability and arguments in the family under COVID-19 restrictions. Questions were answered on a scale from 0 (“A lot less than before”) to 4 (“A lot more than before”) and internal reliability was acceptable (α = 0.78).

Daily screen time use (including gaming and social media use, but not school work) was assessed with a single question, “After the schools closed in March, how many hours of screen time have you had each day?” with the response options “0–1 h”, “2–3 h”, “4–5 h” and “more than 5 h”.

Missing friends and worry about virus infection were assess with two statements, namely, “After the school closed in March, I have missed seeing my friends” and “After the schools closed in March, I have been worried about being infected with the coronavirus or infecting others with the coronavirus”. Items were answered on a scale from 0 (“Not true”) to 2 (“Certainly true”).

As an index of children’s psychological vulnerability, we used their scores on the Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ) [36] and five items from the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Disorders (SCARED) [37] when they first participated in the study (around 18 months earlier). The SMFQ is a 13-item self-report measure of depressive symptoms over the past 2 weeks, while SCARED is a self-report measure of anxiety symptoms over the past 3 months. Both scales have demonstrated good psychometric properties [36,37,38,39] and in the present study, internal reliability was 0.85 and.51 for SMFQ and SCARED, respectively. See Tables 2 and 3 for an overview of the primary variables in the study.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics and correlations among primary variables
Table 3 Descriptive statistics for individual reaction items included in children’s reaction dimensions

Analytic strategy

Data analyses were performed in R [40] using the mice [41], sjPlot [42], polycor [43], miceadds [44] and psych [45] packages. We performed initial item and scale inspections by calculated descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations. We then used a series of t-tests to explore if children’ rated their reactions (at the item level and scale level) as significantly different to before. This was achieved by specifying the mu argument to be equal to two (i.e., the value of the response option corresponding to “As before”). Prior to the main analyses, we imputed missing data on predictors, covariates and outcomes using multiple imputation by chained equations [41]. The percentage of missing data was generally low (< 3%), with the exception of MFQ and SCARED, where missingness was 17%. Seventy-three to 74% of the 442 children would have been available for the main analyses under the traditional listwise deletion method. Five imputed datasets were generated and regression analyses run on each dataset was pooled according to Rubin’s rules [46]. Regression results did not differ when we used incomplete data with listwise deletion or complete cases only, and thus, to utilise all available data we present the results from imputed data. In the main analyses, we performed a series of hierarchical linear regressions with Emotional Reactions, Somatic/cognitive Reactions and Worry Reactions as outcome, respectively, and background variables entered in Step 1, and COVID-19 predictor variables entered in Step 2. Finally, we explored age interactions in a series of regression models, where background variables and COVID-19 predictors were entered in Step 1 and each age interaction entered in Step 2.

Results

Descriptive and preliminary analyses

Table 2 provides means, standard deviations, frequencies and correlations for the primary variables in the study. Unsurprising, the majority of children (78%) reported that they missed their friends and 51.25% worried about being infected or infecting others with the coronavirus. Over half the children reported that their daily screen time usage was four hours or more. Using t-tests we explored if children’s reactions were significantly different to before (i.e., a test value of 2 = “As before”) and found that this was indeed the case for most items, with the exception of “had headache, stomach ache and so on”, “worried about my parents” and “worried about my family’s future” (see Table 3). This suggests that children felt less sad, scared/uneasy, angry and unsafe (as mean scores were < 2), but more lonely and had more difficulty concentrating and slee** at night (as mean scores were > 2) compared to before the government initiated the schools closures. Furthermore, at the scale level, we observed significant results for Emotional Reactions and Somatic/cognitive Reactions, t(440) = -3.57, p < 0.001 and t(440) = 3.01, p < 0.01, respectively. As the average for Emotional Reactions was 1.89, this means that children on average reported fewer emotional reactions compared to before. For Somatic/cognitive Reactions the average was 2.10 and thus, children on average reported more somatic/cognitive reactions compared to before (see Table 2).

Main regression analyses

To explore how children’s Emotional, Somatic/cognitive and Worry reactions, respectively, are associated their experiences and living situation during the pandemic, three hierarchical regression models were estimated (one for each outcome). In the first two models, we found that home school experience and perceived stress and instability in the family during COVID-19 restrictions were associated with children’s Emotional and Somatic/cognitive Reactions, respectively, after controlling for background variables and other predictors in the models. Children who had a more positive home school experiences reported lower Emotional and Somatic/cognitive Reactions, respectively, while children who experienced higher levels of stress and instability in the family reported more Emotional and Somatic/cognitive Reactions. Increased perceived stress and instability in the family was also associated with more Worry Reactions. Furthermore, we found that compared to children who did not miss their friends, those that did miss their friends reported significantly higher Emotional Reactions on average. Finally, compared to children who said they were not worried about virus infection, those that said they were certainly worried about virus infection reported significantly higher Emotional and Worry Reactions, respectively. The three models, respectively, accounted for 46, 33 and 23% of the explained variance in Emotional, Somatic/cognitive and Worry Reactions during lockdown. See Table 4 for an overview of the results. Results were similar when we used the initial dataset with listwise deletion or complete cases only, exception for missing friends, which was non-significant possibly due to a lack of statistical power.

Table 4 Summary of hierarchical regression analyses for variables predicting child reactions (N = 442)

Finally, from our exploration of age as a moderator of the relationship between COVID-19 related predictors and children’s reactions, we found several significant interaction effects after controlling for the main effects of the covariates and other predictors (see Table 5). First, age moderated the association between family stress and all three reaction dimensions. Thus, the older children were the more negatively impacted they were by family stress and instability in terms of their reactions to the new everyday life under COVID-19. Age also moderated the association between screen time use and Somatic/cognitive Reactions with a stronger association for older compared with younger children. Furthermore, we found a significant age by missing friend’s interaction for Somatic/cognitive Reactions, while for Worry Reactions the effects was marginally significant. Thus, the association between age and missing friends was generally stronger for older children when one compared those that missed their friends most to those that did not miss their friends. Finally, we found a significant age by worry about virus infection interaction for Somatic/cognitive Reactions and Worry Reactions. This suggests that for older children, the magnitude of the effect of worry about virus infection on Somatic/cognitive and Worry Reactions is stronger when one compared children who were somewhat worried about virus infection to those that were not worried about virus infections.

Table 5 Summary of hierarchical regression interaction analyses for variables predicting child reactions (N = 442)

Discussion

In this study, we use data from a longitudinal study to shed light on children’s reactions to the new everyday life under COVID-19 with home school and social isolation. We focused on children’s reactions more broadly, as this allowed us to investigate the normal range of reactions and gives a more nuanced view on the implications of the pandemic for children. Specifically, we explored children’s Emotional, Somatic/cognitive and Worry Reactions, and how each of these reaction dimensions relate to variables expected to be pertinent to change under the COVID-19 restrictions.

We asked children how they compared their reactions under COVID-19 with home school and social isolation to before the schools were closed and found that children reported fewer emotional reactions, but more somatic/cognitive reactions. That is, children on average coped better emotionally as they felt less sad, scared, angry and unsafe, but did more poorly in terms of sleep and concentration. That children should experience fewer emotional reactions is in line with certain findings from adults showing only mild levels and no increases in anxiety and depression symptomology [47, 48]. While our results contrast with studies showing increased levels of anxiety and depression in children and adolescents [5, 14, 16]. Children’s worry reactions pertaining to their parents and family were unchanged relative to before, which we tentatively see as positive. Although, we cannot rule out that children had a high level of concern for their family and parents prior and maintained this during the pandemic, as we did not ask children to rate their level of worry per se.

Almost all of the COVID-19 related predictors were associated with children reactions during the pandemic. The strongest predictor of child reactions was children’s perceived family stress and instability, which was significantly and positively associated with all three child-reaction dimensions. Given the interrelatedness of family subsystems [49] and evidenced relationship between child adjustment and general family climate [50], the effects of parent stress and family instability on children under COVID-19 is not surprising. Following Prime and colleagues [26], the effects might have cascading through to children resulting in them reporting more negative reactions (i.e., higher reaction scores) for all three dimensions. We emphasize that in the present study, we controlled for children’s pre-pandemic psychological wellbeing and thus, we have been able to “eliminate” the effect family stress and difficulties usually have on children’s wellbeing. Interestingly, we note that children on average rated the level of family stress and instability to be similar to before the pandemic using our retrospective measure. Why might this be, if families experienced substantial disruptions particularly to family routines and rules? The gravity and extraordinary nature of the COVID-19 situation might somehow have enabled families to mobilize resources to protect the family and its subsystems in the face of pandemic-related stressors [51, 52] or while purely speculative, families might have felt some “comfort” in knowing that they were not alone in their experiences.

Screen time was the only predictor that was not significantly associated with any of the reaction-dimensions. At least one study has found that social media use, but not online gaming, is predictive of loneliness and symptoms of anxiety and depression in youths during pandemic lockdown [20]. We do not know what type of activities children in our study were doing on screens. This, along with study differences in terms of screen time measures and samples, and the fact that we partialled out the effect of pre-pandemic psychological vulnerability might collectively explain the divergent results. During the pandemic, meeting friends digitally was for many children the only opportunity to meet, and thus, the association between screen time and reactions might have been weaker during the pandemic relative to the “normal situation”, where children and adolescents can still meet physically.

We hypothesized that possibly the biggest change for children was the closure of schools and introduction of digital home school. Our results indicate that positive home school experiences acted as a buffer on children’s reactions to the new everyday life; children who did better with home schooling reported fewer emotional and somatic/cognitive reactions. Home schooling likely gave children more autonomy over their day, and it would have required a high level of self-discipline and motivation to establish good routines and succeed with home schooling. One might wonder if children who succeeded at this, were generally well-adjusted and this reflected in their reported reactions. However, our results counter this argument as we controlled for children’s prior psychological wellbeing (or vulnerability), and thus, the observed effect is above and beyond any effect of children’s general adjustment. If a more positive experience of home school has the potential to buffer children against more negative reactions, then school administrations and teachers must ensure that home schooling during a crisis-situation like COVID-19 is optimally put together and delivered to meets students’ needs including teacher support.

Naturally, many children missed their friends and our results indicate that children who missed their friends also reported significantly higher levels of emotional reactions than children who did not miss their friends. Friendships play a large role in children’s lives, not least in terms of a means of support, and our result might indicate that children’s need for emotional support from friends was not met due to the social isolation measures and the absence of usual contact at school, and therefore, they experience heightened emotional reactions.

Finally, our results indicate that children who worried about virus infection reported more emotional and worry Reactions, respectively, compared to children who were not worried about virus infection. This could not be attributed to children’s general emotional vulnerability as we controlled for this in the analyses. Thus, children’s concerns about the coronavirus seem to have direct implications for their emotional response and worries for their family and parents. The association with children’s emotional reactions is particularly important as it demonstrates, for the first time that worry about virus infection is associated with a broader spectre of emotional reactions and not just symptoms of mental health difficulties [5,

References

  1. World Health Organization. Timeline of WHO’s response to COVID-19. 2020 Sep 9th, 2020; Available from: https://www.who.int/news/item/29-06-2020-covidtimeline. [accessed 25 Aug 2020]

  2. Nussbaumer-Streit B et al (2020) Quarantine alone or in combination with other public health measures to control COVID-19: a rapid review. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 9:1–77

    Google Scholar 

  3. Pan A et al (2020) Association of public health interventions with the epidemiology of the COVID-19 outbreak in Wuhan China. JAMA 323(19):1915–1923

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Singh S et al (2002) Impact of COVID-19 and lockdown on mental health of children and adolescents: A narrative review with recommendations. Psychiatry Research 293:113429

  5. Zhou S-J et al (2020) Prevalence and socio-demographic correlates of psychological health problems in Chinese adolescents during the outbreak of COVID-19. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 29(6):749–758

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Ettman CK et al (2020) Prevalence of depression symptoms in US adults before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. JAMA Netw Open 3(9):e2019686

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Shi L et al (2020) Prevalence of and risk factors associated with mental health symptoms among the general population in China during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic. JAMA Netw Open 3(7):e2014053–e2014053

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Lee J (2020) Mental health effects of school closures during COVID-19. Lancet Child Adolesc Health 4(6):421

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Clark H et al (2020) A future for the world’s children? A WHO-UNICEF-Lancet Commission. Lancet 395(10224):605–658

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Allen K et al (2018) What schools need to know about fostering school belonging: a meta-analysis. Educ Psychol Rev 30(1):1–34

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Jose PE, Ryan N, Pryor J (2012) Does social connectedness promote a greater sense of well-being in adolescence over time? J Res Adolesc 22(2):235–251

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Law PC, Cuskelly M, Carroll A (2013) Young people’s perceptions of family, peer, and school connectedness and their impact on adjustment. Aust J Guid Couns 23(1):115–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. O’Rourke J, Cooper M (2010) Lucky to be happy: a study of happiness in Australian primary students. Aust J Educ Dev Psychol 10:94–107

    Google Scholar 

  14. Brazendale K et al (2017) Understanding differences between summer vs. school obesogenic behaviors of children: the structured days hypothesis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 14:100

  15. Engzell P, Frey A, Verhagen MD (2020) Learning inequality during the COVID-19 pandemic

  16. López-Bueno R et al (2021) Potential health-related behaviors for pre-school and school-aged children during COVID-19 lockdown: a narrative review. Prev Med 143:106349

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Bubb S, Jones M-A (2020) Learning from the COVID-19 home-schooling experience: listening to pupils, parents/carers and teachers. Improv Sch 23(3):209–222

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Qvortrup A et al (2020) Nødundervisning under corona-krisen-et elev-og forældreperspektiv. Aarhus Universitetsforlag, Aarhus

    Google Scholar 

  19. Brooks SK et al (2020) The impact of unplanned school closure on children′s social contact: rapid evidence review. Euro Surveill 25(13):2000188

  20. Loades ME et al (2020) Rapid systematic review: the impact of social isolation and loneliness on the mental health of children and adolescents in the context of COVID-19. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 59(11):1218-1239.e3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. López-Bueno R et al (2020) Health-related behaviors among school-aged children and adolescents during the Spanish Covid-19 confinement. Front Pediatr 8:573

  22. **e X et al (2020) Mental health status among children in home confinement during the coronavirus disease 2019 outbreak in Hubei Province, China. JAMA Pediatrics 174(9):898–900

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Ellis WE, Dumas TM, Forbes LM (2020) Physically isolated but socially connected: psychological adjustment and stress among adolescents during the initial COVID-19 crisis. Can J Behav Sci 52(3):177–187

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Bekkhus M, Von Soest T, Fredriksen E (2020) Psykisk helse hos ungdom under covid-19—Ensomhet, venner og sosiale medier. Tidsskrift for Norsk Psykologforening 57(7)

  25. Biroli P et al., Family life in lockdown. IZA Discussion Paper No. 13398, 2020.

  26. Prime H, Wade M, Browne DT (2020) Risk and resilience in family well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. Am Psychol 75(5):631–643

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Walsh F (2015) Strengthening family resilience, 3rd edn. Guilford, New York

    Google Scholar 

  28. Bakken, A., et al., Oslo-ungdom i koronatiden. En studie av ungdom under covid-19-pandemien, in NOVA Rapport 12/2020. Oslo: Norway; 2020.

  29. Lades LK et al (2020) Daily emotional well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. Br J Health Psychol 25(4):902–911

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Bruining H et al (2020) COVID-19 and child and adolescent psychiatry: an unexpected blessing for part of our population? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry [Letter to the editor]

  31. Wade M, Prime H, Browne DT (2020) Why we need longitudinal mental health research with children and youth during (and after) the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychiatry Res 290:113143

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Lau S, Chan DW, Lau PS (1999) Facets of loneliness and depression among Chinese children and adolescents. J Soc Psychol 139(6):713–729

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. De Goede IH, Branje SJ, Meeus WH (2009) Developmental changes in adolescents’ perceptions of relationships with their parents. J Youth Adolesc 38(1):75–88

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Brown BB (2004) Adolescents’ relationships with peers. In: Lerner R, Steinberg L (eds) Handbook of Adolescent Psychology, 2nd edn. Wiley, New York, pp 363–394

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  35. Ministry of Education and Research, Norske skoler var stengt på grunn av korona i ni uker - mot 14 uker i gjennomsnitt i OECD-landene [Press release]. 2020.

  36. Angold A et al (1995) Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 5(4):237–249

    Google Scholar 

  37. Birmaher B et al (1997) The Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED): scale construction and psychometric characteristics. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 36(4):545–553

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Sharp C, Goodyer IM, Croudace TJ (2006) The Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ): a unidimensional item response theory and categorical data factor analysis of self-report ratings from a community sample of 7-through 11-year-old children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 34(3):365–377

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Birmaher B et al (1999) Psychometric properties of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED): a replication study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 38(10):1230–1236

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. R Core Team (2019) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria https://www.r-project.org/

  41. Van Buuren S, Groothuis-Oudshoorn K (2011) mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. J Stat Softw 45(3):1–68

  42. Lüdecke D (2019). sjPlot: Data Visualization for Statistics in Social Science. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1308157, R package version 2.7.0, https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=sjPlot

  43. Fox J (2019) polycor: Polychoric and Polyserial Correlations. R package version 0.7-10. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=polycor

  44.  Robitzsch A, Grund S (2020) Miceadds: some additional multiple imputation functions, especially for 'mice'. R package version 3.10-28. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=miceadds

  45. Revelle W (2020) PsychTools: tools to accompany the 'psych; package for psychological research. Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois. R package version 2.0.8, https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psychTools

  46. Rubin DB (2004) Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys, vol 81. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  47. Russell B et al (2020) Initial challenges of caregiving during COVID-19: Caregiver burden, mental health, and the parent–child relationship. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 51(5):671–682

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. van der Velden PG et al (2020) Anxiety and depression symptoms, and lack of emotional support among the general population before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. A prospective national study on prevalence and risk factors. J Affect Disord 277:540–548

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Minuchin S, Fishman HC (1981) Family therapy techniques. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  50. Browne DT et al (2015) Cumulative risk and developmental health: an argument for the importance of a family-wide science. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 6(4):397–407

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Calhoun LG, Tedeschi RG (2014) Handbook of posttraumatic growth: Research and practice. Psychology Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  52. Masten AS, Narayan AJ (2012) Child development in the context of disaster, war, and terrorism: Pathways of risk and resilience. Annu Rev Psychol 63:227–257

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. King DL et al (2020) Problematic online gaming and the COVID-19 pandemic. J Behav Addict 9(2):184–186

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all the families who have participated in the FamilieForSK-study and to all the children who participated in the “corona-round” of the FamilieForSK-study and whose data the present study is based on.

Funding

Open access funding provided by Norwegian Institute of Public Health (FHI). The authors were supported by research grants from the Research Council of Norway (grant number: 250642) and the Norwegian Directorate for Children, Youth and Family Affairs (Bufdir) during the preparation of this article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

LL, MSH and TH designed the study, formulated the research questions and drafted the manuscript. LL performed the data analyses.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Linda Larsen.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethics approval

The Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics in Norway approved the study, and have, therefore, been performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments.

Consent to participate

Parents consented for children to participate in the FamilieForSK-study and children assented before completing the online survey or before being interviewed by trained interviewers.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Larsen, L., Helland, M.S. & Holt, T. The impact of school closure and social isolation on children in vulnerable families during COVID-19: a focus on children’s reactions. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 31, 1–11 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-021-01758-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-021-01758-x

Keywords