Log in

The tendency for experiencing involuntary future and past mental time travel is robustly related to thought suppression: an exploratory study

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Involuntary mental time travel (MTT) refers to projecting oneself into the past or into the future without prior conscious effort. The previous studies have shown high inter-individual variability in the frequency of involuntary MTT, but a few systematic studies exist. In three exploratory studies, we investigated the relation between individual differences in experiencing involuntary past and future MTT, and selected emotional and cognitive processes, with a special focus on thought suppression. Across all three studies, thought suppression emerged as a robust predictor of involuntary MTT above and beyond emotion-related variables, mind-wandering, daydreaming styles, and demographic variables. Findings from Studies 1 and 2 showed that higher thought suppression consistently predicted both more frequent involuntary past and future MTT across an American and a Danish sample, whereas rumination and emotion regulation were less consistently related to involuntary MTT. In Study 3, thought suppression reliably predicted more frequent involuntary MTT, even when controlling for mind-wandering, as well as for positive and negative daydreaming styles, which were all related to greater involuntary MTT. Overall, the individual differences assessed showed similar relationships to the tendency for having past and future involuntary MTT, with the possible exception of daydreaming styles, which appeared more strongly related to future-directed involuntary MTT.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price includes VAT (France)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data sets during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

Notes

  1. The following steps were taken to increase the reliability of the data collection process. First, the entire survey consisted of 275 single items, and thus, it was determined that spending a minimum of 8 min answering the survey was a mandatory inclusion criterion (1.5 s per item plus instructions). Note that some of the questionnaires in the survey were not analyzed for the present study; however, the 8-min rule was based on the entire set of questionnaires completed by participants. Second, an attention check was inserted in the IAMI collected via Amazon MTurk (“If you are reading this item attentively, select the last option ‘Once an hour or more’”). Of the original 510 participants, 42 (8.2%) completed the survey in less than 8 min. Forty additional participants (7.8% of the 510) did not pass the attention check, thus resulting in N = 428.

  2. The entire survey consisted of 275 single items. All Danish participants took 8 min or more in the survey (N = 481); thus, we did not eliminate anybody because of speed. We did not employ an attention check for the Danish participants; thus, data from all 481 Danish participants were analyzed. The data from this sample derived from two other larger projects and, thus, the questionnaires were given in two different orders. (Analyses not overlap** with the present study are published in del Palacio-Gonzalez & Berntsen, 2018b.)

  3. The data preparation for Study 3 followed the same steps as Study 1. With 127 items, we estimated that the minimum time required to answer the survey was 4 min. Note that the entire survey was shorter than that of Studies 1 and 2, and we analyzed data from all questionnaires. Initially, 567 participants accessed the survey, of which 7.9% (n = 45) were too fast. An additional 5.6 % of the original (n = 32) did not pass the attention check, thus leaving a final sample of 490 participants for analysis.

  4. The variance corresponds to the ΔR2 and sr2 explained by the MWQ in an additional model in which the MWQ was entered alone after the SIPI subscales. The model is not reported in the manuscript, but is available upon request.

References

  • Aldao, A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2010). Specificity of cognitive emotion regulation strategies: A transdiagnostic examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(10), 974–983.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alle, M., Berna, F., & Berntsen, D. (2018). Involuntary autobiographical memory and future thought predicting hallucination proneness. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(6), 891–898.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Antrobus, J. S., Singer, J. L., Goldstein, S., & Fortgang, M. (1970). Mind-wandering and cognitive structure. Transactions of the New York Academy of SCIence (Series 11), 32, 242–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaty, R. E., Seli, P., & Schacter, D. L. (2018). Thinking about the past and future in daily life: an experience sampling study of individual differences in mental time travel. Psychological Research. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1075-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berntsen, D. (1996). Involuntary autobiographical memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10(5), 435–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berntsen, D. (2009). Involuntary autobiographical memories: An introduction to the unbidden past. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Berntsen, D. (2018). Spontaneous future cognitions: A theoretical review, Psychological Research. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1127-z

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berntsen, D., & Bohn, A. (2010). Remembering and forecasting: The relation between autobiographical memory and episodic future thinking. Memory and Cognition, 38(3), 265–278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berntsen, D., & Jacobsen, A. S. (2008). Involuntary (spontaneous) mental time travel into the past and future. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 17(4), 1093–1104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berntsen, D., Rubin, D. C., & Salgado, S. (2015). The frequency of involuntary autobiographical memories and future thoughts in relation to daydreaming, emotional distress, and age. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 36, 352–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buhrmester, M. D., Talaifar, S., & Gosling, S. D. (2018). An evaluation of Amazon’s Mechanical Turk, its rapid rise, and its effective use. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 149–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. NeurosCIence, Nature Reviews 17, 718–731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (2010). Cognitive therapy of anxiety disorders: SCIence and practice. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, S. N., & Berntsen, D. (2016). Do future thoughts reflect personal goals? Current concerns and mental time travel into the past and future. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 273–284.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, S. N., Staugaard, S., & Berntsen, D. (2016). Inducing involuntary and voluntary mental time travel using a laboratory paradigm. Memory and Cognition, 44(3), 376–389.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 12(11), 671–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, G. (2009). Inference by eye: Reading the overlap of independent confidence intervals. Statistics in MediCIne, 28, 205–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Argembeau, A. (2012). Autobiographical memory and future thinking. In D. Berntsen & D. C. Rubin (Eds.). Understanding autobiographical memory: Theories and approaches (pp. 311–330). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Argembeau, A., & Van der Lindeen, M. (2006). Individual differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: The effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies. ConsCIousness and Cognition, 15, 342–350.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., & Berntsen, D. (2018a). Emotional responses to everyday mental time travel in social anxiety: A naturalistic study. (Manuscript in preparation).

  • del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., & Berntsen, D. (2018b). Emotion regulation of events central to identity and their relationship with concurrent and prospective depressive symptoms. Behavior Therapy, 49, 604–616.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., Berntsen, D., & Watson, L. A. (2017). Emotional intensity and emotion regulation in response to autobiographical memories during dysphoria. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 41(4), 530–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erskine, J. A. K., Kvavilashvili, L., & Kornbrot, D. E. (2007). The predictors of thought suppression in young and old adults: Effects of rumination, anxiety, and other variables. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(6), 1047–1057.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finnbogadóttir, H., & Berntsen, D. (2011). Involuntary and voluntary mental time travel in high and low worriers. Memory, 19(6), 625–640.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Finnbogadóttir, H., & Berntsen, D. (2013). Involuntary future projections are as frequent as involuntary memories, but more positive. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 22(1), 272–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giambra, L. M. (1993). The influence of aging on spontaneous shifts of attention from external stimuli to the contents of consciousness. Experimental Gerontology, 28, 485–492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and SoCIal Psychology, 85(2), 348–362.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grysman, A. (2015). Collecting narrative data on amazon’s mechanical turk. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29(4), 573–583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hintzman, D. L. (2011). Research strategy in the study of memory: Fads, fallacies, and the search for the “coordinates of truth”. Perspectives on Psychological SCIence, 6(3), 253–271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huba, G. J., Segal, B., & Singer, J. L. (1977). Consistency of daydreaming styles across samples of college male and female drug and alcohol users. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86(1), 99–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huba, G. J., Singer, J. L., Aneshenel, C. S., & Antrobus, J. S. (1982). Short Imaginal Process Inventory. In Short Imaginal Porcesses Inventory. Manual. London: Research Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyman, I. E. Jr., Cutshaw, K. I., Hall, C. M., Snyders, M. E., Masters, S. A., Au, V. S. K., & Graham, J. M. (2015). Involuntary to intrusive: Using involuntary musical imagery to explore individual differences and the nature of intrusive thoughts. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25(1), 14–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, S. R., & Fernyhough, C. (2009). Rumination, reflection, intrusive thoughts, and hallucination-proneness: Towards a new model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(1), 54–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kamiya, S. (2014). Relationship between frequency of involuntary autobiographical memories and cognitive failure. Memory, 22(7), 839–851.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H. W., Soetens, B., Braet, C., & De Raedt, R. (2008). How to control a white bear? Individual differences involved in self-perceived and actual thought-suppression ability. Cognition and Emotion, 22(6), 1068–1080.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krans, J., de Bree, J., & Moulds, M. L. (2015). Involuntary cognitions in everyday life: Exploration of type, quality, content, and function. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6, 11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lavy, E. H., & Van, dH. (1990). Thought suppression induces intrusions. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 18(4), 251–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mace, J. H. (2007). Involuntary memory: Concept and theory. In J. H. Mace (Ed.), Involuntary memory; involuntary memory (pp. 1–19). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Marchetti, I., Koster, E. H. W., Klinger, E., & Alloy, L. B. (2016). Spontaneous thought and vulnerability to mood disorders: The dark side of the wandering mind. Clinical Psychological SCIence, 4(5), 835–857.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McMillan, R. L., Kaufman, S. B., & Singer, J. L. (2013). Ode to positive constructive daydreaming. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, T., Otgaar, H., & Smeets, T. (2015). Flashbacks, intrusions, mind-wandering - instances of an involuntary memory spectrum: A commentary on Takarangi, Strange, and Lindsay (2014). ConsCIousness and Cognition, 33, 24–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miles, L. K., Karpinska, K., Lumsden, J., & Macrae, C. N. (2010). The meandering mind: Vection and mental time travel. PLoS One, 5(5), e10825.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mrazek, M. D., Phillips, D. T., Franklin, M. S., Broadway, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Young and restless: Validation of the mind-wandering questionnaire (MWQ) reveals disruptive impact of mind-wandering for youth. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., & Horselenberg, R. (1996). Individual differences in thought suppression. the white bear suppression inventory: Factor structure, reliability, validity and correlates. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(5–6), 501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., & Grayson, C. (1999). Explaining the gender difference in depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality and SoCIal Psychology, 77(5), 1061–1072.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palacio-Gonzalez, A., Clark, D. A., & O’Sullivan, L.,F. (2017). Cognitive processing in the aftermath of relationship dissolution: Associations with concurrent and prospective distress and posttraumatic growth. Stress and Health, 33(5), 540–548.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plimpton, B., Patel, P., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2015). Role of triggers and dysphoria in mind-wandering about past, present and future: A laboratory study. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 33, 261–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, A. S., & Berntsen, D. (2011). The unpredictable past: Spontaneous autobiographical memories outnumber autobiographical memories retrieved strategically. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 20(4), 1842–1846.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, A. S., & Berntsen, D. (2013). The reality of the past versus the ideality of the future: Emotional valence and functional differences between past and future mental time travel. Memory & Cognition, 41(2), 187–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, A. S., Ramsgaard, S. B., & Berntsen, D. (2015). Frequency and functions of involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories across the day. Psychology of ConsCIousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 2(2), 185–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, D. C., Dennis, M. F., & Beckham, J. C. (2011). Autobiographical memory for stressful events: The role of autobiographical memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. ConsCIousness and Cognition, 20(3), 840–856.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Salkovskis, P. M., & Campbell, P. (1994). Thought suppression induces intrusion in naturally occurring negative intrusive thoughts. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(1), 1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salkovskis, P. M., & Reynolds, M. (1994). Thought suppression and smoking cessation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(2), 193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews NeurosCIence, 8(9), 657–661.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic memory. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Risko, E. F., Smilek, D., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Mind-wandering with and without intention. Trends in Cognitive SCIences, 20(8), 605–617.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. L. (1974). Daydreaming and the stream of thought. American SCIentist, 62(4), 417–425.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. L. (1975). Navigating the stream of consciousness: Research in daydreaming and related inner experience. American Psychologist, 30(7), 727–738.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., & Andrews-Hanna, J. (2013). Not all minds that wander are lost: The importance of a balanced perspective on the mind-wandering state. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946–958.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 487–518.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smets, J., Wessel, I., Schreurs, E., & Raes, F. (2012). The interplay between rumination and intrusions in the prediction of concurrent and prospective depressive symptoms in two nonclinical samples. The Psychological Record, 62(4), 777–788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szpunar, K. K. (2010). Episodic future thought: An emerging concept. Perspectives on Psychological SCIence, 5(2), 142–162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, E. R. (2007). Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(2), 227–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turk, I. R. Rise, and its effective use. Perspectives on Psychological SCIence. 13, 149–154.

  • Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 163–206.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., I.,II, & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and SoCIal Psychology, 53(1), 5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression. Journal of Personality, 62(4), 615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This work was supported by the Danish National Research Foundation (DNRF) under Grant DNRF89. The DNRF had no involvement in the design of the current study or the interpretation of the results.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Adriana del Palacio-Gonzalez.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The author declares that there is no competing interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., Berntsen, D. The tendency for experiencing involuntary future and past mental time travel is robustly related to thought suppression: an exploratory study. Psychological Research 83, 788–804 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1132-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1132-2

Navigation