Still Falling at the First Hurdle: Examining Early Grade Reading in South Africa

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
South African Schooling: The Enigma of Inequality

Part of the book series: Policy Implications of Research in Education ((PIRE,volume 10))

Abstract

This chapter provides an overview of what we know about reading outcomes in South Africa. After an initial survey of some foundational tenets of reading research we show that while reading outcomes in South Africa improved between 2006 and 2011 they have stagnated between 2011 and 2016. The most recent PIRLS study (2016) showed that 78% of Grade 4 children cannot read for meaning in any language. There is nothing inevitable about these results. The knowledge and instructional practices required to teach children to read – as well as the resources needed to do it – are known and well understood internationally, even in high-poverty contexts. We argue that the inequalities evident in the schooling system have their roots in unequal life chances doled out at birth and consolidated through differential reading trajectories. Moving beyond the ‘comprehension iceberg’ we document what lies beneath these dire results. The majority of children have not mastered the basics of decoding in their home language in Grade 1 or 2 making reading for meaning or pleasure unlikely. We advocate an approach focusing on early reading success and ensuring that teachers know how to teach reading, that they have the materials to do so, that children have ready access to books and that reading outcomes are assessed annually.

The title Falling at the first hurdle was that of a research report by Taylor (1989) on literacy in South African schools. That we face similar challenges with similar diagnoses 30 years on is reason enough to reproduce Taylor’s incisive title, with a slight modification.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free ship** worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free ship** worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1.

    In kee** with official South African curriculum terminology, we use the terms Home Language and Additional Language. In the literature more broadly, these are synonymous with first language (L1) and second language (L2) learning.

  2. 2.

    While SACMEQ has released its 2013 results and claimed they are comparable, they have not released any technical documentation or data as is standard practice in previous rounds of SACMEQ (Ross et al. 2005), and in other international assessments (see the 300+ page technical reports for TIMSS, PISA or PIRLS for example). Because there are open and unanswerable questions around their validity (Spaull 2016b), notably that the assessment instruments used and the analytical assumptions made changed between 2007 and 2013, we do not discuss the SACMEQ 2013 time trends in reading.

  3. 3.

    A technical note of some importance is that older reports of the prePIRLS 2011 results (Howie et al. 2012; Mullis et al. 2012) use a different scale to the traditional PIRLS scale. This was because the prePIRLS assessments were not calibrated to be equated to PIRLS in 2011. This was rectified with the release of the 2016 PIRLS results where the International Association for the Evaluation of Education (IEA) retrospectively rescaled the prePIRLS scores to be comparable to the PIRLS scores. Thus while in 2011 one could not compare PIRLS-2006 and prePIRLS-2011, by 2016 one could compare PIRLS-2006, prePIRLS-2011 and PIRLS-Literacy-2016 all on the same PIRLS scale (as in Table 8.2 below). All three included nationally representative samples of Grade 4 learners who were assessed in whatever the language of learning and teaching was used in that school in Grades 1–3. (Note prePIRLS and PIRLS-Literacy are easier versions of PIRLS that use texts of approximately 400 words rather than the 800 word texts of PIRLS, although for equating purposes there are two PIRLS passages in PIRLS-Literacy and two PIRLS-Literacy passages in PIRLS (Mullis and Martin 2015, p. 28)).

  4. 4.

    We do not report the Grade 5 results from PIRLS 2011 or PIRLS 2016 since these assessments were not administered to a nationally-representative sample of primary schools. They were only administered to English- and Afrikaans-LOLT schools in 2011 and English, Afrikaans and isiZulu-LOLT schools in 2016.

  5. 5.

    The oft-cited 40-point figure for a year of learning is based on three Nordic countries which each assessed two consecutive grades in PIRLS; namely 3rd and 4th Grade in Sweden, and 4th and 5th Grade in Iceland and Norway. The overall differences were found to be 41 points in Sweden, 39 points in Iceland and 43 points in Norway (Rosén 2010, p. 7). The more correct 50-point figure comes from the South African PIRLS experience in 2006 where a nationally-representative sample of Grade 4 and Grade 5 learners from the same schools were assessed at the same time and on the same assessment yielding a 49-point difference (Howie et al. 2008, p. 19).

  6. 6.

    As an aside, it is also worth noting that the improvement in performance between 2006 and 2011 is not undisputed. For example, the official PIRLS 2016 report indicates that the trend results for South Africa are only comparable between 2011 and 2016 and that between 2006 and 2011 the data is “not comparable for measuring trends to 2016, primarily due to countries improving translations or increasing population coverage” (Mullis et al. 2017: 303). In the case of South Africa this is primarily because in the PIRLS 2006 assessment, the psychometric scales and instruments were not calibrated to measure performance accurately below 300 PIRLS points (Personal Communication, Dirk Heystedt (2017)). In 2006 South Africa’s score was 253. This may be an underestimate due to motivation problems where learners become demotivated by encountering texts that are far too difficult to them. Notwithstanding the above, it is highly unlikely that the full improvement from 2006 to 2011 is accounted for by motivation problems alone rather than a real improvement in reading outcomes.

  7. 7.

    School wealth here is calculated as the average of student asset wealth in the school. Student wealth is calculated using Multiple Correspondence Analysis (MCA) on the eight possession questions in PIRLS Literacy 2016 (PIRLS, 2018a: S1.1; 2018b: 2,7). While this is unlikely to create an accurate cardinal indicator of wealth, the purpose here is to create an ordinal ranking and this is arguably the best measure of student wealth available. Calculations on the PIRLS Low International Benchmark use the first plausible value.

  8. 8.

    According to V-ANA (2013), 90% of fee-charging schools in the sample had English or Afrikaans as their language of assessment. In addition, given that virtually all independent schools are either English or Afrikaans medium and fee-charging, this adds a further 4–5% of learners to this group.

  9. 9.

    Although this benchmark is derived from learning to read in English, all languages that use the Roman alphabet in their orthography should reflect fairly similar benchmarks.

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Learning and thinking about print. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alderson, J. C. (2005). Diagnosing foreign language proficiency: The interface between learning and assessment. London: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aro, M., & Wimmer, H. (2003). Learning to read: English in comparison to six more regular orthographies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(4), 621–635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biemiller, A. (2012). Teaching vocabulary in the primary grades: Vocabulary instruction needed. In E. Kame’enui & J. Baumann (Eds.), Vocabulary instruction: Research to practice (pp. 34–50). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, B. W., & Saks, D. H. (1986). Measuring the effects of instructional time on student learning: Evidence from the beginning teacher evaluation study. American Journal of Education, 94(4), 480–500. https://doi.org/10.1086/443863

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castles, A., Rastle, K., & Nation, K. (2018). Ending the reading wars: Reading acquisition from novice to expert. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 19(1), 5–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chall, J. S., Jacobs, V. A., & Baldwin, L. E. (1990). The reading crisis: Why poor children fall behind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corson, D. (1997). The learning and use of academic English words. Language Learning, 47(4), 671–718.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). Early reading acquisition and its relation to reading experience and ability 10 years later. Developmental Psychology, 33(6), 934.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain: The new science of how we read. London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Department of Basic Education. (2014). Second Detailed Indicator Report for Basic Education Sector. Pretoria.

    Google Scholar 

  • Department of Basic Education. (2018). NEIMS Standard Reports January 2018. National Education Infrastructure Management System. Pretoria.

    Google Scholar 

  • EGRS. (2018). Early grade Reading study (EGRS I & II). Pretoria: Department of Basic Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farkas, G., & Beron, K. (2004). The detailed age trajectory of oral vocabulary knowledge: Differences by class and race. Social Science Research 33(3), 464–497. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2003.08.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleisch, B. (2008). Primary education in crisis: Why South African school children underachieve in reading and mathematics. Cape Town: Juta.

    Google Scholar 

  • Good, R. H., Simmons, D. C., & Kame’enui, E. J. (2001). The importance and decision-making utility of a continuum of fluency-based indicators of foundational reading skills for third-grade high-stakes outcomes. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5(3), 257–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gxilishe, D. (1996). The dilemma of dialect in the classroom: A case for Xhosa. Per Linguam, 12(1), 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H Brookes Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (2003). The early catastrophe: The 30 million word gap by age 3. American Educator, 27(1), 4–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helman, L. A., & Burns, M. K. (2008). What does oral language have to do with it? Hel** young english-language learners acquire a sight word vocabulary. The Reading Teacher, 62(1), 14–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hernandez, D. J. (2011). Double jeopardy: How third-grade reading skills and poverty influence high school graduation. Baltimore: Annie E Casey Foundation. http://www.aecf.org.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heystedt, D. (2017). Personal Communication. 1 December 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoadley, U. (2012). What do we know about teaching and learning in South African primary schools? Education as Change, 16(2), 187–202. https://doi.org/10.1080/16823206.2012.745725

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2(2), 127–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howie, S., Venter, E., Van Staden, S., Zimmerman, L., Long, C., Du Toit, C., Scherman, V., & Archer, E. (2008). Progress in International Reading Literacy Study 2006. University of Pretoria: Centre for Evaluation and Assessment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howie, S. J., Mokoena, G., Dowse, C., & Zimmerman, L. (2012). PIRLS 2011: Progress in international reading literacy study 2011: South African children’s reading literacy achievement. Centre for Evaluation and Assessment (CEA), University of Pretoria.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howie, S., Van Staden, S., Tshele, M., Dowse, C., & Zimmerman, L. (2017). Progress in international reading literacy study 2016. South African children’s reading literacy achievement. Summary Report. Centre for Evaluation and Assessment, Pretoria.

    Google Scholar 

  • ITERP. (2014). An examination of aspects of initial teacher education curricula at five higher education institutions. Initial Teacher Education Research Project. Johannesburg: JET Education Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski, R. A., & Good III, R. H. (1996). Toward a technology for assessing basic early literacy skills. School Psychology Review, 25(22), 215–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, J. (2011). Size matters: Early vocabulary as a predictor of language and literacy competence. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32(1), 69–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lesnick, J., Goerge, R., Smithgall, C., & Gwynne, J. (2010). Reading on grade level in third grade: How is it related to high school performance and college enrollment (Vol. 1, p. 12). Chicago: Chapin Hall at the University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipka, O., & Siegel, L. S. (2007). The development of reading skills in children with english as a second language. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(2), 105–131. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430709336555

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malda, M., Nel, C., & van de Vijver, F. J. (2014). The road to reading for South African learners: The role of orthographic depth. Learning and Individual Differences, 30, 34–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11(3), F9–F16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mouton, J. (2017). Evaluation of Zenex literacy project. Commissioned by the Zenex Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mtsatse, N. (2017). Exploring differential item functioning on reading achievement between English and isiXhosa. Unpublished MA dissertation, University of Pretoria, Pretoria.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullis, I. V., Martin, M. O., Kennedy, A. M., Trong, K. L., & Sainsbury, M. (2009). PIRLS 2011 assessment framework. International association for the evaluation of educational achievement. ERIC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullis, I. V. S., & Martin, M. O. (2015). PIRLS 2016 assessment framework international association for the evaluation of educational achievement (2nd ed.). Boston: TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Foy, P., & Drucker, K. T. (2012). PIRLS 2011 international results in reading. Boston: TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Foy, P., & Hooper, M. (2017). PIRLS 2016: International results in reading. Amsterdam: International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., & Diethelm, K. (2001). The contribution of phonological skills and letter knowledge to early reading development in a multilingual population. Language Learning, 51(2), 187–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • NEEDU. (2013). NEEDU National Report 2012: The state of literacy teaching and learning in the foundation phase. National education and evaluation development unit. Department of Basic Education, Pretoria.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pikulski, J. J., & Templeton, S. (2004). Teaching and develo** vocabulary: Key to long-term reading success. In Current research in reading/language arts (pp. 1–12). Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co. http://www.eduplace.com.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1997). Forward. In D. McGuinness (Ed.), Why our children can’t read, and what we can do about it: A scientific revolution in reading. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M. (2006). Reading instruction that works: The case for balanced teaching. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pretorius, E. (2019). Getting it right from the start: Some cautionary notes for early reading instruction in African languages. In N Spaull & JP Comings (Eds.), Improving Early Literacy Outcomes: Curriculum, Teaching and Assessment (pp.63–80). Leiden: IBE/BRIL. https://doi.org/10.1163/9789004402379_005

    Google Scholar 

  • Pretorius, E. J., & Spaull, N. (2016). Exploring relationships between oral reading fluency and reading comprehension amongst English second language readers in South Africa. Reading and Writing, 29(7), 1449–1471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Probert, T. (2016). A comparative study of syllables and morphemes as literacy processing units in word recognition: IsiXhosa and Setswana. Unpublished MA dissertation, Rhodes University, Grahamstown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rees, S. (2016). Morphological awareness in readers of isiXhosa. Unpublished MA dissertation, Rhodes University, Grahamstown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reeves, C., Heugh, K., Prinsloo, C. H., Macdonald, C., Netshitangani, T., Alidou H, Diedericks, G., & Herbst, D. (2008). Evaluation of literacy teaching in primary schools of Limpopo province. HSRC http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.11910/4984

  • Ross, K. N., Dolata, S., Ikeda, M., Zuze, L., & Murimba, S. (2005). The conduct of the SACMEQ II project in Kenya. Harare: Southern and Eastern African Consortium for Monitoring Education Quality.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosén, M. (2010). On the degree of comparability in trend studies as a function of differences in age and schooling. Paper presented at the IEA IRC, Sweden. Retrieved from http://www.iea.nl/sites/default/files/irc/IRC2010_Rosen_Strietholt

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. (2001). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis) abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In S. Neuman, D. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 97–110). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. (2017). Language at the speed of sight: How we read, why so many can’t, and what can be done about it. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., Ouellette, G., & Rodney, D. (2006). The misunderstood giant: On the predictive role of early vocabulary to future reading. Handbook of Early Literacy Research, 2, 173–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shalem, Y. (2003). Do we have a theory of change? Calling change models to account. Perspectives in Education, 21(1), 29–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spaull, N. (2013). Poverty & privilege: Primary school inequality in South Africa. International Journal of Educational Development, 33(5), 436–447. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijedudev.2012.09.009

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spaull, N. (2015). Accountability and capacity in South African education. Education as Change, 19(3), 113–142. https://doi.org/10.1080/16823206.2015.1056199

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spaull, N. (2016a). Excessive class sizes in the foundation phase. Policy Brief Research on Socioeconomic Policy (RESEP)(Online) Available: www.resep.sun.ac.za

  • Spaull, N. (2016b). Shaky data skews literacy results. https://mg.co.za/article/2016-09-23-00-shaky-data-skews-literacy-results/

    Google Scholar 

  • Spaull, N., Pretorius, E. J., & Mohohlwane, N. (2018). Investigating the comprehension iceberg: Develo** empirical benchmarks for early grade reading in agglutinating African languages. Stellenbosch University. RESEP working paper (Series no. WP01/2018). Available: www.resep.sun.ac.za.

  • Staehr, L. S. (2008). Vocabulary size and the skills of listening, reading and writing. Language Learning Journal, 36(2), 139–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, K., & Stahl, S. (2012). Young word wizards! Fostering vocabulary development in preschool and primary education. In E. J. Kameenui & J. F. Baumann (Eds.), Vocabulary instruction: Research to practice (pp. 72–92). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (2000). Progress in understanding reading: Scientific foundations and new frontiers. London: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, N. (1989). Falling at the first hurdle: Initial encounters with the formal system of African education in South Africa. University of the Witwatersrand, Education Policy Unit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, N., & Vinjevold, P. (1999). Getting learning right: Report of the president’s education initiative research project. Braamfontein: Joint Education Trust

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. (2019). How can learning inequalities be reduced? Lessons learnt from experimental research in South Africa. In N. Spaull & J. Jansen (Eds.), South African schooling: The enigma of inequality. New York: Springer Nature. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-18811-5_1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, N. (2019). Inequalities in teacher knowledge in South Africa. In N. Spaull & J. Jansen (Eds.), South African schooling: The enigma of inequality. New York: Springer Nature. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-18811-5_1

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E., Chapman, J. W., & Prochnow, J. E. (2006). Literate cultural capital at school entry predicts later reading. New Zealand Journal of Educational Studies, 41(2), 183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E., Chapman, J. W., Greaney, K. T., Prochnow, J. E., & Arrow, A. W. (2013). Why the New Zealand national literacy strategy has failed and what can be done about it: Evidence from the progress in international reading literacy study (PIRLS) 2011 and reading recovery monitoring reports. Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties, 18(2), 139–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Berg, S., Burger, C., Burger, R., de Vos, M., du Rand, G., Gustafsson, M., Moses, E., Shepherd, D. L., Spaull, N., Taylor, S., van Broekhuizen, H., & von Fintel, D. (2011). Low quality education as a poverty trap. Research Report for the PSPPD project for presidency. ID 2973766, University of Stellenbosch, Department of Economics, Stellenbosch.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vasilyeva, M., & Waterfall, H. (2011). Variability in language development: Relation to socioeconomic status and environmental input. Handbook of Early Literacy Research, 3, 36–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R. (1991). Introduction to three studies on reading acquisition: Convergent findings on theoretical foundations of code-oriented versus whole-language approaches to reading instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(4), 437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69(3), 848–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wills, G. (2016). An economic perspective on school leadership and teachers’ unions in South Africa. Thesis, Stellenbosch University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilsenach, C. (2015). Receptive vocabulary and early literacy skills in emergent bilingual Northern Sotho-English children. Reading & Writing-Journal of the Reading Association of South Africa, 6(1), 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. (2008). Proust and the squid: The story and science of the reading brain (reprint edition ed.). New York: Harper Perennial.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nic Spaull .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Spaull, N., Pretorius, E. (2019). Still Falling at the First Hurdle: Examining Early Grade Reading in South Africa. In: Spaull, N., Jansen, J. (eds) South African Schooling: The Enigma of Inequality. Policy Implications of Research in Education, vol 10. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-18811-5_8

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-18811-5_8

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-18810-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-18811-5

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics

Navigation