Externally Modeling Mental Models

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Learning and Instructional Technologies for the 21st Century

Abstract

Meaningful learning, as opposed to reproductive learning, is active, constructive, intentional, authentic, and collaborative. When learners engage in meaningful learning, they naturally construct mental models. When learners collaborate, they naturally construct group mental models. One method for engaging learners in meaningful learning is to have them construct computer-based models that externalize their mental models. Using tools such as databases, concept maps, expert systems, spreadsheets, systems modeling tools, microworlds and simulation tools, teachable agents, computer conferences, and hypermedia, learners can construct models of domain knowledge, problems, systems, semantic structures, and thinking processes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (Canada)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (Canada)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (Canada)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free ship** worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (Canada)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free ship** worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams-Webber, J. (1995). Constructivist psychology and knowledge elicitation. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 8(3), 237–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barley, S. R., & Bechty, B. A. (1994). In the backrooms of science: The work of technicians in science labs. Work and Occupations, 21(1), 85–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (1997). Creativity: Shifting across ontological categories flexibly. In T. B. Ward, S. M. Smith, & J. Vaid (Eds.), Conceptual Structures and processes: Emergence, Discovery and Change. (pp. 209–234). Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Confrey, J., & Doerr, H. M. (1994). Student modelers. Interactive Learning Environments, 4(3), 199–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiSessa, A., & Abeson, H. (1986). Boxer: A reconstructible computational medium. Communications of the ACM, 29, 859–868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dole, J. A., Sinatra, G. M. (1998). Reconceptualizing change in the cognitive construction of knowledge. Educational Psychologist, 33, 109–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durkheim, É. (1915) The elementary forms of the religious life (J. W. Swain, Trans.). New York and London: The Free press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engeström, Y. (1987). Learning by expanding: An activity theoretical approach to developmental research. Helsinki, Finland: Orienta-Konsultit Oy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frederiksen, J. R., & White, B. Y. (1998). Teaching and learning generic modeling and reasoning skills. Journal of Interactive Learning Environments, 55, 33–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Stevens, A. L. (1983). Mental models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D. H. (1997). Instructional design models for well-structured and ill-structured problem-solving learning outcomes. Educational Technology: Research and Development, 45(1), 65–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D. H. (2000). Computers as Mindtools for schools: Engaging critical thinking. Columbus, OH: Merrill/Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D. H. (2003). Using cognitive tools to represent problems. Journal of Research on Technology in Education, 35(3), 362–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D. H., Beissner, K., & Yacci, M. A. (1993). Structural knowledge: Techniques for representing, conveying, and acquiring structural knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D. H., & Henning, P. (1999). Mental models: Knowledge in the head and knowledge in the world. Educational Technology, 39(3), 37–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kraiger, K., & Salas, E. (1993, April). Measuring mental models to assess learning during training. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Industrial/Organizational Psychology, San Francisco, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J. H. (1983). The role of problem representation in physics. In D. Gentner & A. L. Stevens (Eds.). Mental models (pp. 75–98). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2000). Modeling in mathematics and science. In R. Glaser (Ed.) Advances in instructional psychology: volume 5. Educational design and cognitive science (pp. 101–159). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lippert, R. C. (1988). An expert system shell to teach problem solving. Tech Trends, 33(2), 22–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGuinness, C. (1986). Problem representation: The effects of spatial arrays. Memory & Cognition, 14(3), 270–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mellar, H., Bliss, J., Boohan, R., Ogborn, J., & Tompsett, C. (1994). Learning with artificial worlds: Computer-based modeling in the curriculum. London: Falmer Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penner, D. E., Giles, N. D., Lhrer, R., & Schauble, L. (1997). Building functional models: designing and elbow. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 34(2), 125–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ploetzner, R., Fehse, E., Kneser, C., & Spada, H. (1999). Learning to relate qualitative and quantitative problem representations in a model-based setting for collaborative problem solving. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 8(2), 177–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ploetzner, R., & Spada, H. (1998). Constructing quantitative problem representations on the basis of qualitative reasoning. Interactive Learning Environments, 5, 95–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rips, L. J. (1986). Mental muddles. In M. Brand & R.M. Harnish (Eds.), The representation of knowledge and beliefs (pp. 258–286). Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G., Perkins, D. N., & Globerson, T. (1991). Partners in cognition: Extending human intelligence with intelligent technologies. Educational Researcher, 20(3), 2–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Savelsbergh, E. R., De Jong, T., & Ferguson-Hessler, M. G. M. (1998). Competence related differences in problem representations. In M. van Someren, P. Reimann, T. de Jong, & H. Boshuizen (Eds.), The role of multiple representations in learning and problem solving. (pp. 263–282).Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. (1994). Goal-based scenarios. In R. C. Schank & E. Langer (Eds.), Beliefs, reasoning, and decision making: Psycho-logic in honor of Bob Abelson. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, C., & White, B. (2005). Meta-modeling knowledge: Develo** students′ understanding of scientific modeling. Cognition and Instruction, 23(2), 165–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, J. L., & Yerulshalmy, M. (1987). The geometric supposer: Using microcomputers to restore invention to the learning of mathematics. In D. Perkins, J. Lockhead, & J. C. Bishop (Eds.), Thinking: The second international conference (pp. 525–536). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shavelson, R. J. (1972). Some aspects of the correspondence between content structure and cognitive structure in physics instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 225–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spector, J. M., Christensen, D. L, Sioutine, A. V, & McCormack, D. (2001, September-November) Models and simulations for learning in complex domains: Using causal loop diagrams for assessment and evaluation. Computers in Human Behavior, 17(5–6), 517–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1992). Spatial mental models derived from survey and route descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 261–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Veer, G. C. (1989). Individual differences and the user interface. Ergonomics, 32(11), 1431–1449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, B. (1993). ThinkerTools: Causal models, conceptual change, and science education. Cognition and Instruction, 10(1), 1–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1922). Tractatus logico-philosophicus. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David H. Jonassen .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2009 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Jonassen, D.H. (2009). Externally Modeling Mental Models. In: Moller, L., Huett, J., Harvey, D. (eds) Learning and Instructional Technologies for the 21st Century. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-09667-4_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics

Navigation