Introduction

There are various structural and functional differences in brain architecture between the left and right hemispheres1,2,3,4,5,6. Microstructural brain characteristics, such as white matter (WM) pathways or intra- and extra-neurite water organisation, might underlie the brain’s functional lateralisation7. Furthermore, handedness has been repeatedly assessed together with asymmetry in humans and animals, and relates to brain asymmetry8. Both structural and functional brain asymmetry exhibit clinical importance as there are differences in brain asymmetry between healthy controls and various disease groups, including neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease9,10, Parkinson’s disease11, and psychiatric disease such as obsessive-compulsive disorder12,13 and schizophrenia14. In that context and particularly relevant from a lifespan-perspective, cortical thickness asymmetry decreases throughout ageing, with this alteration being potentially accelerated in the development of neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer’s Disease9. Similarly, some studies suggest lower WM microstructure asymmetry at higher ages, indicated by intra-axonal water fraction15, fractional anisotropy, or the apparent diffusion coefficient16. Additional investigations into brain asymmetries’ age-dependencies can provide a more comprehensive understanding of the influence of asymmetries on ageing and disease development.

Brain age is a develo** integrative marker of brain health, particularly sensitive to neurodegenerative diseases17,18. Brain age refers to the predicted age in contrast to chronological age and is based on a set of scalar metrics derived from brain scans such as MR. To date, brain age has often been estimated using a global brain parametrisation such as the averaged scalar measures over particular anatomical regions or the whole brain17,18,19,20,21. Hence, we refer to these whole-brain age predictions as global brain age (GBA). However, while brain age has been calculated for different brain regions18,22,23,24, the use of hemisphere-specific data is usually not being considered as a potential source of additional information. Yet, one study presents hemisphere-specific and region-specific brain ages containing useful clinical information about post-stroke cognitive improvement22.

Previous results show that brain age prediction depends on the specific features used25,26,27, rendering for example modality as important. Yet, the influence of hemispheric differences or brain asymmetry on the age predictions remains unclear. However, previously outlined brain asymmetries1,2,3,4,5,6 might be informative for age predictions. One way of leveraging brain asymmetries into simple metrics is to estimate separate brain ages for each hemisphere (HBA) and to then compare the estimates. It remains unclear whether predictions from a single hemisphere lead to less accurate predictions due to the inclusion of less data and a potential attenuation of noise. At the same time, in the case of diffusion MRI (dMRI), different model-based diffusion features yield highly concordant brain age predictions, also when varying the number of included features21. Finally, although the evidence is mixed on the influence of handedness on brain asymmetry28,29,30,31, differences in handedness are potentially reflected in brain structure, which would in turn influence age predictions differently when obtained from the left or right hemisphere only. Hence, handedness requires further examination as potential confounding effect when assessing asymmetry.

HBA, a new brain age measure, may propose more sensitive brain health markers than GBA, as age predictions can be compared between hemispheres to infer the integrity of each hemisphere and give a general estimate of brain asymmetry. Brain asymmetries are commonly observed using the Laterality Index (LI)32. However, different ways of estimating asymmetry can introduce variability in its dependency with age33, and covariates of brain age require further investigation34,35. To extend the existing brain age conceptualisation of using features across the whole brain and to maximise interpretability, we restrict brain age predictions to region-averaged and global features and not asymmetries of these features. Additionally, differences in the models’ abilities to predict age from WM microstructure features derived from dMRI compared to T1-weighted features (volume, surface area, thickness) need to be ruled out in order to validate both GBA and HBA.

Hence, in the present work, we tested first the preregistered hypotheses (written study and analysis plan prior data inspection and analyses36,37) that the GBA and HBA depend on the used MRI modality (Hypothesis 1), disentangling whether the different grey matter (GM) and WM metrics and the degree of their asymmetry influences brain age predictions. We furthermore tested whether there was an effect of hemisphere (Hypothesis 2) and handedness (Hypothesis 3) on brain age predictions. Exploratory analyses included (a) revealing hemispheric differences between GM and WM features, (b) examining LI associations with age, including the LI of the brain features as well as left and right brain ages, and (c) testing the consistency of brain age-covariate associations (specifically, health-and-lifestyle factors, as these were previously associated with brain age20,26,38,39,40,41).

Results

Hemispheric differences and age sensitivity for GM and WM features

Two-tailed paired samples t-tests showed that a significant proportion of the GM and WM features differed between hemispheres with medium effect sizes. Among the significant 793 of 840 dMRI feature asymmetries (94.4%, padj< 0.05, with Cohen’s \(| {\bar{d}}_{dMRI}|\) = 0.57 ± 0.44). The largest differences were found for DTI FA in the inferior longitudinal fasciculus (d = 3.64), and cingulum (d = 1.95), and for AD in superior longitudinal fasciculus.

Effects sizes of the significant hemispheric differences of the 115 of 117 T1-weighted features (98.3%), were similar: mean \(| {\bar{d}}_{{T}_{1}}|\) = 0.53 ± 0.41, and the largest asymmetries were found for the surface area of the transverse-temporal region (d = 1.81), frontal pole (d = 1.76), and pars orbitalis (d = 1.74; see Supplementary Table 10 for T1-weighted and dMRI features with strongest hemispheric differences).

Likelihood Ratio Tests (LRTs) comparing a baseline model predicting age from sex and scanner site compared to a model where the respective smooth of the metric was added (Eq. (3) and (4)) indicated most features as age-sensitive (231 of the 234 (98.72%) of the T1-weighted features; 1601 of the 1680 (95.53%) dMRI features). Age-sensitivity was strongly expressed in both significant T1-weighted features (\({\bar{F}}_{{T}_{1}}\) = 1168.90 ± 993.59), as well as significant dMRI metrics (\({\bar{F}}_{dMRI}\) = 1208.97 ± 943.52) with strongest age-sensitivity observed for left superior temporal thickness, left/right overall thickness, left/right hippocampus volume, and right inferior parietal thickness and multiple WMM metrics in the right anterior limb of the internal capsule, the left/right fornix-striaterminalis pathway, left/right anterior corona radiata and inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (F > 3000; for top features see Supplementary Table 2). Results were similar when comparing linear models to the baseline model (Eq. (2) and (4)): 1448 of the 1680 (86.19%) dMRI metrics, and 228 of the 234 (97.44%) of the T1-weighted features were age-sensitive (\({\bar{F}}_{{T}_{1}}\) = 3426.89 ± 2947.11, \({\bar{F}}_{dMRI}\) = 2378.46 ± 2357.80), with the features with the strongest age-sensitivity resembling LRT results of non-linear models (for top features see Supplementary Table 3).

Considering only left/right averages identified only DTI-AD, and WMTI axial and radial extra-axonal diffusivity to not differ between hemispheres (padj >0.05). Furthermore, all features were age-sensitive when GAMs (padj < 3.4 × 10−64; yet for linear models, BRIA-vCSF and WMTI-axEAD, as well as right DTI-AD and left WMTI-radEAD were not age sensitive (Supplementary Tables 4, 5). Furthermore, the age-relationships for most of the left/right averages were similar across hemispheres (Fig. 1, both for crude and adjusted values: Supplementary Fig. 1, and for linear and non-linear models: Supplementary Fig. 4). However, differences in dMRI metrics were observed for the ends of the distribution including individuals aged younger than 55 (N = 5307) and older than 75 (N = 3480).

Fig. 1: Age curves of standardized and zero-centered mean values of GM and WM features per hemisphere.
figure 1

A cubic smooth function (s) with k = 4 knots was applied to plot the relationship between age and brain features correcting for sex and scanner site (F): \(\hat{age}=s(F)+sex+site\) using restricted maximum likelihood (REML). The grey shaded area indicates the 95% CI. All age-relationships were significant (padj < 3.4 × 10−64). Sample sizes for diffusion metrics: NdMRI = 39, 637, sample size for T1 metrics: \({N}_{{T}_{1}}=48,040\). Source data are provided in Source Data file 1.

GM and WM feature asymmetry

Using LRTs comparing GAMs to a baseline model 53 (45.30%) of the 117 T1-weighted and 733 of the 840 (87.26%) dMRI ∣LI∣ features as age sensitive (padj< 0.05). Using LRTs on linear effects identified 53 (45.30%) of the 117 T1-weighted and 678 of the 840 (80.71%) dMRI ∣LI∣ features as age sensitive (padj< 0.05).

In the following we constrain analyses to linear models and present partial derivatives/slopes as a measure of effect size, allowing for simple comparisons across age-relationships as model fit indices AIC and BIC of linear models and GAMs suggested on average no differences across both T1-weighted (padj AIC = 0.759; padj BIC = 1) and diffusion-weighted features (dAIC = 0.510, padj AIC = 0.020; padj BIC = 0.126).

The absolute feature asymmetries were higher later in life (\({\bar{\beta }}_{dMRI}=0.05\pm 0.07\); \({\bar{\beta }}_{{T}_{1}}=0.03\pm 0.06\), \(| {\bar{\beta }}_{multimodal}|=0.05\pm 0.07\), here only padj < 0.05 selected; Supplementary Figs. 2, 3).

The strongest adjusted relationships between the respective features’ asymmetries and age were found for dMRI metrics (\(| {\bar{\beta }}_{dMRI}|\) = 0.06 ± 0.04, \(| {\bar{\beta }}_{{T}_{1}}|\) = 0.05 ± 0.03; Fig. 2), particularly outlining asymmetry increases in the tapetum (βSMTmcintra = 0.24, βBRIAVintra = 0.24, βSMTFA = 0.23) and fornix-stria terminalis (βDTIMD = 0.22, βBRIAVcsf = 0.21), and decrease in the tapetum (βSMTlong = −0.23), cerebral peduncle (βSMTmcextratrans = −0.20, βSMTtrans = −0.19, βBRIAVextra = −0.16) and superior longitudinal temporal fasciculus (βSMTlong = −0.14).

Fig. 2: T1-weighted and dMRI features linear asymmetry-age-associations.
figure 2

The plot presents the standardized (sex- and site-corrected) regression slopes versus Bonferroni-adjusted -log10 p-values. Modelling was done using Eq. (2): \(\hat{age}={\beta }_{0}+{\beta }_{1}\times F+{\beta }_{2}\times Sex+{\beta }_{3}\times Site\), where F is the respective brain feature. Labelling was done separately for T1-weighted and dMRI indicating the 10 most significantly associated features (five for β > 0 and five for β < 0). ILF inferior longitudinal fasciculus, Cereb.Peduncle Cerebral peduncle, Rostro-mid. thickness Rostro-middle thickness, SLFT Superior longitudinal fasciculus (temporal part), Fornix-Str.Term. Fornix-stria terminalis tract, Caud. ant. cingulate Caudal anterior cingulate. Sample sizes for diffusion metrics: NdMRI = 39,637, sample size for T1 metrics: \({N}_{{T}_{1}}=48,040\). Source data are provided in Source Data file 2.

For T1-weighted metrics, larger, and central structures’ ∣LI∣ were most sensitive to age, with the strongest negative associations including the inferior lateral (β = −0.15) and lateral ventricles (β = −0.10), pallidum (β = −0.12) volumes, rostro-middle thickness (β = −0.11), thalamus volume (β = −0.09) and enthorinal area (β = −0.06). Largest positive age-associations were were shown for accumbens area (β = 0.13), WM surface area (β = 0.12), amygdala volume (β = 0.11), caudal anterior cingulate thicknes (β = 0.11), cortical (β = 0.09) and white matter volume (β = 0.09), as well as caudate volume (β = 0.09), in addition to several temporal and limbic areas (Fig. 2).

Sex-specific differences in the influence of hemisphere, modality, and handedness on brain age estimates

Model performance metrics indicated that most accurately age predictions were accomplished using multimodal MRI data based on left, right, and both hemispheres (Table 1), with obtained HBA and GBA being strongly correlated with each other for similar models (Fig. 3). Additional sex-stratified models produced similar results in terms of model performance (Supplementary Table 14), associations across brain ages and age (Supplementary Fig. 10, and feature importance rankings (compare Supplementary Tables 1113).

Table 1 Hemispheric brain age prediction outcomes
Fig. 3: Pearson correlation coefficients between chronological and predicted ages for T1-weighted, diffusion, and multimodal MRI for left, right and both hemispheres, N = 35,665.
figure 3

All Bonferroni-corrected p < 0.001. L Left hemisphere, R Right hemisphere, LR Both hemispheres. Source data are provided in Source Data file 3.

LMERs did not indicate a difference between modalities (Hypothesis 1) when comparing brain ages estimated from both sexes from dMRI to multimodal MRI (p = 0.623), and dMRI to T1-weighted MRI (p = 0.452). There were also no differences in brain age estimates between hemispheres (p = 0.413, Hypothesis 2). Moreover, LRTs indicated no significant difference between models when adding handedness (χ2 = 4.19, p = 0.123, df = 2) or handedness-hemisphere interaction and handedness (χ2 = 7.32, p =0.120, df = 4; see Eqs. (5)–(6)).

To additionally consider sex differences, we estimated additional sex-specific brain ages and control for the modelling choice (as extension to Eq. (6)). We find that females’ brain ages do not differ when estimated from females’ data only compared to predictions from both males’ and females’ data (β = −0.0073 years, p = 0.420). The same holds true for male brain ages estimated from males’ data only compared to data from both sexes (β = − 0.0002 years, p = 0.984). Furthermore, with these additional modelling choices, we identified a significant marginal effect of sex (indicating an older brain age for males: β = 0.58 years, p < 0.001), and hemisphere for T1-weighted (β = 0.03 years, p = 0.022), but not dMRI (β = 0.02 years, p = 0.099), or multimodal MRI (β = 0.02 years, p = 0.110). Moreover, ambidextrous brain age was higher than for left-handed (β = 1 year, p < 0.001) and right handed participants (β = 0.7 years, p < 0.001), as well as higher for right-handed compared to left-handed participants (β = 0.2 years, p < 0.001).

Further investigating the identified sex-effect, we found higher brain ages for males across modalities with larger differences identified for dMRI (βleft = 0.768 years, p < 0.001, βright = 0.870 years, p < 0.001), followed by T1-weighted (βleft = 0.308 years, p < 0.001, βright = 0.438 years, p < 0.001) and multimodal MRI (βleft = 0.503 years, p < 0.001, βright = 0.570 years, p < 0.001). Notably, females’ right brain age was lower than the left brain age (\({\beta }_{{T}_{1}}=-0.035\,years,p=.027\), βdMRI = − 0.029 years, p = 0.066, βmultimodal = −0.013 years, p = 0.403), which was the opposite for males showing lower left brain age (\({\beta }_{{T}_{1}}=0.095\,years,p < .001\), βdMRI = 0.073 years, p < 0.001, βmultimodal = − 0.054 years, p = 0.001). In contrast to the analyses across sexes, these additional analyses provide support for Hypotheses 1-3 when sex-stratifying.

Lower brain age asymmetry at higher ages

To test whether asymmetries between hemisphere-specific brain age predictions are lower at higher age, ∣LIHBA∣, was associated with age (Eq. (7)–(8)). ∣LIHBA∣ showed negative unadjusted associations with age for T1-weighted (r = −0.069, p < 0.001), dMRI (r = −0.121, p < 0.001), and multimodal models (r = −0.121, p < 0.001). The associations were similar when using LMEs adjusting for sex and the random intercept site (T1-weighted: β = − 0.069, p < 0.001, dMRI: β = −0.115, p < 0.001, multimodal: β = −0.117, p < 0.001). LRTs indicate the age-sensitivity of LIHBA (T1-weighted: χ2 = 173.42, p < 0.001, dMRI: χ2 = 488.74, p <0.001, multimodal: χ2 = 506.08, p < 0.001).

These results were robust to stratifying by sex, estimates from a brain age model considering both sexes for unadjusted (rdMRI males = −0.134, rdMR Ifemales = −0.104, rT1 males = −0.134, rT1 females = −0.048, rmultimodal males = −0.134, rmultimodal females = −0.111), and adjusted associations (βdMRI males = −0.134, βdMRI females = −0.099, βT1 males = −0.134, βT1 females = −0.045, βmultimodal males = −0.134, βmultimodal females = −0.106), with χ2 tests suggesting age sensitivity (all p < 0.001).

Using brain age predictions from models which were independently estimated for males and females showed similar results for unadjusted (rdMRI males = − 0.141, rdMRI females = − 0.094,rT1 males = − 0.120, rT1 females = − 0.031, rmultimodal males = −0.165, rmultimodal females = −0.089), and adjusted associations (βdMRI males = −0.137, βdMRI females = − 0.088, βT1 males = −0.117, βT1 females = −0.029, βmultimodal males = −0.162, βmultimodal females = −0.084), with χ2 tests suggesting age sensitivity (all p < 0.001).

Finally, also when analysing brain ages for males and females from sex-specific models together shows similar trends for uncorrected ∣LIHBA∣-age associations (rmultimodal = −0.123, p < 0.001; \({r}_{{T}_{1}}=-0.074\), p < 0.001, rdMRI = − 0.114, p < 0.001), as well as corrected association (βmultimodal = −0.125, p < 0.001; \({\beta }_{{T}_{1}}=-0.071\), p < 0.001, βdMRI = −0.113, p < 0.001; Eq. (7)–(8)).

HBA and GBA and health-and-lifestyle factors

We further investigated the pattern of relationships with general health-and-lifestyle phenotypes across HBAs (Fig. 4). Relationships between brain ages from single and both hemispheres were similar within modalities, but varied slightly between modalities (Fig. 4). These results were robust to sex stratifications. Yet, while males’ brain age was sensitive to high cholesterol, hip circumference, smoking and weight, this was not the case for females’ brain age when using brain age predictions from data of both sexes (Supplementary Figure 1112).

Fig. 4: Linear association between general health-and-lifestyle phenotypes and brain age estimated from different modalities, left, right and both hemispheres.
figure 4

Eq. (9) was used and standardized slopes are presented. For simplicity, standardized slopes with ∣β∣ < 0.005 were rounded down to β = 0. L Left hemisphere, R right hemisphere, LR Both hemispheres, BMI Body mass index, WHR Waist-to-hip ratio. Bonferroni-adjusted p < 0.05 is marked by a black frame. Sample size: N = 35,665. Missingness in predictors: NAlcohol = 237, NBMI = 1247, NDiastolic Blood Pressure = 4442, NHeight = 1189, NHip Circumference = 1153, NPulse Pressure = 4442, NSmoking = 339, NSmoking Status = 224, NSystolic Blood Pressure = 4442, NWaist Circumference = 1154, NWeight = 1212, NWHR = 1154. Source data are provided in Source Data file 4.

Sex stratified hemispheric differences and age sensitivity for GM and WM features

For further insights into sex differences, we repeated the presented analyses on hemispheric differences and features’ age-sensitivity stratifying by sex. Two-tailed paired samples t-tests assessing regional differences between hemispheres showed similar results between sexes, which are also comparable to cross-sex results. Most features differed between hemispheres for both males and females (T1-weighted: 98.3% for both sexes, dMRImales: 96%, dMRIfemales: 95%), and effect sizes were similar (\(| {\bar{d}}_{{T}_{1}males}|\) = 0.54 ± 0.42, \(| {\bar{d}}_{{T}_{1}females}|\) = 0.53 ± 0.42, \(| {\bar{d}}_{dMRImales}|\) = 0.57 ± 0.41, \(| {\bar{d}}_{dMRIfemales}|\) = 0.60 ± 0.47).

Also the strongest effects were similar across sexes: strongest differences in T1-weighted features in males were observed for frontal pole (\({d}_{{T}_{1}males}\) = 1.82) and pars orbitalis (\({d}_{{T}_{1}males}\) = 1.78) surface area, and for females in the area of the transverse temporal area (\({d}_{{T}_{1}females}\) = 1.89) and the frontal pole (\({d}_{{T}_{1}females}\) = 1.73). Strongest WM differences were observed for both sexes in inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ddMRI males = 3.44, ddMRI females = 3.91), and superior longitudinal temporal fasciculus (ddMRI males = 2.09, ddMRI females = 2.40; Supplementary Table 6).

LRTs comparing a baseline model predicting age from sex and scanner site compared to a model where the respective smooth of the metric was added (Eq. (3) and (4)) indicated most features as age-sensitive (230 of the 234 (98.29%) of the T1-weighted features (both sexes); 1557 and 1564 of the 1680 (92.68% and 93.10%) dMRI features for males and females, respective). Age-sensitivity was strongly expressed in both significant T1-weighted features (\({\bar{F}}_{{T}_{1}males}\) = 640.80 ± 521.33; \({\bar{F}}_{{T}_{1}females}\) = 578.61 ± 500.79), as well as significant dMRI metrics (\({\bar{F}}_{dMRImales}\) = 586.38 ± 450.68, \({\bar{F}}_{dMRIfemales}\) = 674.61 ± 499.58).

Similar to the results including both sexes, the strongest T1-weighted feature age-sensitivity was observed for left superior temporal thickness, left/right hippocampus volume for both sexes, and right inferior parietal thickness only for females. Concerning dMRI features, sex stratification reflects the findings accounting for sex, outlining the fornix-striaterminalis pathway, anterior corona radiata and inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, yet adding the anterior limb of the internal capsule and the anterior thalamic radiation. Unique to non-linear models, also the lateral ventricle volume was lined out as highly age sensitive (all F > 1666; for top features see Supplementary Table 7.

Results were similar when comparing linear models to the baseline model (Eq. (2) and (4)): 1557 and 1564 of the 1680 (92.68%, 93.01%) dMRI metrics, and 226 and 224 of the 234 (96.58%, 95.73%) of the T1-weighted features were age-sensitive for males and females, respectively (\({\bar{F}}_{{T}_{1}males}\) = 1767.60 ± 1474.69; \({\bar{F}}_{{T}_{1}females}\) = 1712.73 ± 1488.97; \({\bar{F}}_{dMRImales}\) = 1198.85 ± 1135.84, \({\bar{F}}_{dMRIfemales}\) = 1297.51 ± 1257.02), with the features with the strongest age-sensitivity resembling LRT results of non-linear models (for top features see Supplementary Table 8).

Considering only left and right hemispheric averages, t-tests indicated that all features differed between hemispheres for males (padj < 3.1 × 10−9). In females, WMTI radEAD and axEAD as well as DTI AD did not differ between hemispheres (padj > 0.05), but all other metrics differing between hemispheres (padj < 1.5 × 10−36).

Considering all regional features, LRTs on GAMs (Eq. (4), (3)) indicated that all features were age-sensitive (padj < 5.1 × 10−71). LRTs on linear models (Eq. (2), (4)) indicated that right hemisphere BRIA-vCSF and left microRD were not age sensitive (padj > 0.05) in males. In females, additionally, left DTI-RD and GM thickness as well as left and right WMTI-axEAD were not age-sensitive. All other metrics were age sensitive (padj < 2.7 × 10−11). Hemispheric features’ age-relationships showed similar intercepts and slopes across sexes, except DTI-AD, WMTI-radEAD and WMTI-axEAD (Supplementary Figs. 5, 6).

Sex differences in GM and WM feature asymmetry

Sex-stratified analyses indicate most dMRI ∣LI∣ features to be age sensitive (dMRImales = 64.29%, dMRIfemales = 69.52%), but less T1-weighted features (T1 males = 47.86%, T1 females = 38.46%) when using non-linear models. Linear models showed similar results (dMRImales = 60.95%, dMRIfemales = 64.05%; T1 males = 44.44%, T1 females = 37.61%). Comparing linear to non-linear models using two-sided paired samples t-tests suggests no differences model fit indicated in AIC or BIC scores for both males and females in T1-weighted and diffusion features’ asymmetry (padj > 0.05). Hence, linear model outcomes are presented below. Similar to models including both sexes, when stratifying for sex, ∣LI∣ for diffusion and T1-weighted feature were positively associated with age (\({\bar{\beta }}_{dMRImale}=0.05\pm 0.08\), \({\bar{\beta }}_{dMRIfemale}=0.05\pm 0.08\), \({\bar{\beta }}_{T1male}=0.03\pm 0.06\), \({\bar{\beta }}_{T1female}=0.03\pm 0.06\)).

The strongest adjusted relationships for diffusion features were found in the cingulate gyrus tract (βmales BRIAmicroRD = 0.25, βmales BRIAmicroFA = 0.22, βfemales BRIAmicroRD = 0.25, βmales BRIAmicroFA = 0.21) and in the cerebral peduncle (βmales SMTmcextratrans = −0.19, βmales SMTtrans = − 0.18, βfemales SMTmcextratrans = −0.21, βfemales SMTtrans = −0.20, βfemales BRIAVextra = −0.18; Supplementary Figs. 8, 9). Strongest age associations with T1-weighted asymmetries were found for the area of the accumbens (βmales = 0.14, βfemales =0.12) and WM surface (βmales =0.13, βfemales = 0.12), with strongest inverse relationships observed for inferior lateral ventricles (βmales = − 0.17, βfemales = − 0.14) and pallidum (βmales = −0.11, βfemales = − 0.12).

Discussion

In the present work we investigated a new way of utilising brain age to differentiate between hemispheres, and performed a detailed assessment of brain asymmetry associations with age. As a baseline, we showed that most grey and white matter features were age-sensitive and differed between hemispheres with relatively large effect sizes. Brain asymmetry was age-sensitive, and overall higher at higher ages. In contrast, asymmetry in hemispheric brain age was lower at higher ages. The strongest relationship of age and absolute brain asymmetry was identified in larger GM and WM regions, as well subcortical and lower structures, including the limbic system, the ventricles, cingulate and cerebral as well as cerebral peduncle WM.

Brain age predictions exhibited concordant accuracy within modalities for left, right, and both hemispheres, and concordant associations with health-and-lifestyle factors also when analysing data for males and females separately, training brain age models on data from each sex separately or both sexes together. The predictions did not differ statistically between hemispheres, modalities, or handedness groups when considering both sexes together. However, sex-stratified analyses, which considered different brain age modelling choices, revealed significant opposing effects between sexes for hemisphere and modality, and outlined marginal differences between handedness groups. There are multiple reasons for the observed higher brain age in females’ right hemisphere compared to males’ higher brain age of the left hemisphere, in addition to modality-specific differences. First, male and female brain structure differs, resulting in sex-specific regional variations in brain age estimates42. Second, body and brain ageing trajectories differ between sexes, for example, outlined by sex-dependent importance of cardiometabolic risk factors43. Hence, the tendency of males’ predicted brain age being lower using T1-weighted and multimodal in contrast to diffusion-derived brain ages, with these trends reversed in females, might also reflect stronger brain age associations with cardiometabolic risk factors in males (Supplementary Fig. 7), which have been demonstrated earlier for WM features and WM brain age38,39. HBA allows to assess the structural integrity of each hemisphere individually, and to set brain ages from the two hemispheres in relationship to each other providing a general marker of asymmetry. Despite brain asymmetries overall increasing (Supplementary Figs. 2, 3), the asymmetries between left/right HBA were smaller at a higher age. At higher ages, both hemispheres might hence become overall more comparable, despite ageing-related changes44.

We found that the majority of regional and hemisphere-averaged MRI features differed between hemispheres. Both features and asymmetries were age-sensitive indicating that the investigation of asymmetries are useful across ages and MRI modalities.

Interestingly, hemisphere-averaged features’ age-associations and HBA of the same modality were similar between hemispheres (Fig. 1), and the hemisphere was not a significant predictor of brain age estimated from a particular hemisphere, when analysing data from both sexes together. However, when sex-stratifying, modality and hemisphere were significant predictors, suggesting that HBA captures both brain asymmetries as well as biological sex-differences which become apparent when using multimodal MRI. These results outline the importance of considering sex-differences in brain age analyses.

Several studies present evidence for asymmetries in WM6,45,46,47,48 and GM4,9,49,50,51. In contrast to these previous studies, for the first time, we examine various metrics supplying information on both WM and GM in a large sample. While we find various differences between hemispheres, age relationships of T1-weighted and dMRI features were similar between hemispheres using hemispheric averages, also when stratifying by sex. Spatially finer-grained examinations revealed more specific patterns of asymmetry in T1-weighted features, such as GM thickness9, and dMRI features45. This is also shown in the present study by stronger age-effects for specific regional asymmetries compared to asymmetries in hemispheric averages. Age-MRI metric relationships depend, however, on the selected metric, the sample, and the sampling (cross-sectional or longitudinal)52,53. For example, previous evidence from T1-weighted MRI indicates no differences in GM volume between hemispheres54, but hemispheric differences of cortical thickness and surface area across ageing4,9.

The presented age charts of MRI metrics in the current work (Fig. 1, Supplementary Fig. 1) provide similar trends to those reported in previous studies observing global age dependencies19,21,55,56,57. Yet, the stratification between hemispheres when presenting brain features’ age dependence is a novel way of presenting brain charts.

We found asymmetries based on GM and WM brain scalar measures. Unimodal studies with smaller, younger samples presented age-dependence of the brain asymmetry during early WM development48 and adult cortical thickness9, other T1-derived metrics33, and functional network development5, showing lower asymmetry at higher ages. In contrast to HBA asymmetries, brain asymmetries do generally not support the notion of lower but instead of higher brain asymmetry later in life. Different study design choices, such as temporal and spatial levels might provide supplemental information into the age-dependence of brain asymmetries, for example, by further investigating longitudinal and voxel-level asymmetries.

We extended previous findings by providing a comprehensive overview of brain asymmetry associations throughout mid- to late life including both GM and WM. Our findings indicate that when considering various metrics, older brains generally appear less symmetric than younger brains in the current sample mid- to late-life sample, whereas brain age appears more symmetric in older brains.

Notably, we identified strong associations between specific brain regions’ asymmetry and age. The strongest age associations of asymmetries were observed for subcortical, ventricle-near structures. The general age-sensitivity of such structures21,58,4,9,54. We identified lower asymmetry linked to higher ages in the ventricular and pallidum volumes, appearing alongside the known effect of larger ventricle volumes at higher ages55. The strongest positive age-relationships for T1-weighted features’ asymmetry were observed for accumbens and WM surface area, as well as limbic structures such as amygdala, hippocampus, and cingulate. Limbic structures have previously been outlined as highly age-sensitive21,58,91, Gibbs ringing92, susceptibility-induced and motion distortions, and eddy current artifacts93. Isotropic 1 mm3 Gaussian smoothing was carried out using FSL’s94,95fslmaths. Employing the multi-shell data, Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI)96, Diffusion Kurtosis Imaging (DKI)97 and White Matter Tract Integrity (WMTI)98 metrics were estimated using Matlab 2017b code (https://github.com/NYU-DiffusionMRI/DESIGNER). Spherical mean technique (SMT)99, and multi-compartment spherical mean technique (SMTmc)100 metrics were estimated using original code (https://github.com/ekaden/smt)99,100. Estimates from the Bayesian Rotational Invariant Approach (BRIA) were evaluated by the original Matlab code (https://bitbucket.org/reisert/baydiff/src/master/)101.

T1-weighted images were processed using Freesurfer (version 5.3)102 automatic recon-all pipeline for cortical reconstruction and subcortical segmentation of the T1-weighted images (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki)103.

In total, we obtained 28 WM metrics from six diffusion approaches (DTI, DKI, WMTI, SMT, SMTmc, BRIA; see for overview in Supplement 9). In order to normalise all metrics, we used Tract-based Spatial Statistics (TBSS)104, as part of FSL94,95. In brief, initially all brain-extracted105 fractional anisotropy (FA) images were aligned to MNI space using non-linear transformation (FNIRT)95. Following, the mean FA image and related mean FA skeleton were derived. Each diffusion scalar map was projected onto the mean FA skeleton using the TBSS procedure. In order to provide a quantitative description of diffusion metrics we used the John Hopkins University (JHU) atlas106, and obtained 30 hemisphere-specific WM regions of interest (ROIs) based on a probabilistic WM atlas (JHU)107 for each of the 28 metrics. For T1-weighted data, we applied the Desikan-Killiany Atlas108. Altogether, 840 dMRI features were derived per individual [28 metrics × (24 ROIs + 6 tracts)] for each hemisphere, and 117 T1-weighted features (surface area, volume, thickness for each of the 34 regions; 3 whole-brain gray matter averages, and 2 averages of white matter surface area and volume) for each hemisphere.

Brain age predictions

Brain age was predicted using the XGBoost algorithm109 implemented in Python (v3.7.1). We used six data subsets to predict brain age split in the following manner: 1) right hemisphere T1-weighted, 2) left hemisphere T1-weighted, 3) left hemisphere diffusion, 4) right hemisphere diffusion, 5) left hemisphere multimodal, 6) right hemisphere multimodal. We applied nested k-fold cross-validation with 5 outer and 10 inner folds (see Supplementary Table 1 for tuned hyperparameters for models trained on data from both sexes together and Supplementary Table 15 for models trained separately for males and females). We corrected for age-bias and mere age-effects110,111 by including age in the regression equations (Eq. (5)) when assessing effects of modality, hemisphere, and handedness on brain age, as well as phenotype associations with brain ages (Eq. (9)).

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were carried out using Python (v3.7.1) and R (v4.2.0).

Hemispheric differences and age sensitivity

To give an overview of the extent of brain asymmetry, we assessed the significance of T1-weighted and dMRI features’ asymmetry using two-sided t-tests. The lateralisation or asymmetry of the brain features was estimated as the following: we applied the LI32 to both regional features and features averaged over each hemisphere (see also ref. 33).

$$LI=\frac{L-R}{L+R},$$
(1)

where L and R belongs to any left and right scalar metric, respectively. Furthermore, when associating LI with age, we used absolute LI values (∣LI∣) allowing to estimate age-effects on asymmetry irrespective of the direction of the asymmetry (leftwards or rightwards).

We then used linear regression models correcting for sex and scanning site to predict age from all regular and LI features:

$$\hat{Age}=F+Sex+Site,$$
(2)

where F is a scalar metric such as, for example, hippocampus volume (derived from T1-weighted image) or tapetum fractional anisotropy (derived from DTI). The same model setup was used applying generalised additive models (GAM) to model non-linear relationships between F and Age using a smooth s of linked quadratic functions with k = 4 knots and restricted maximum likelihood (REML):

$$\hat{Age}=s(F)+Sex+Site.$$
(3)

Likelihood ratio tests (LRTs)112 were used to assess the age sensitivity of all T1-weighted and dMRI features and their asymmetry/LI features by comparing the above models with baseline models not including the respective feature:

$$\hat{Age}=Sex+Site.$$
(4)

We used the same procedure for region-averaged and hemispheric average metrics for regular and LI features. Hemispheric averages of regular features were then visualised by age, including surface area, volume, thickness for T1-weighted data, and intra- and extra-axonal water diffusivities as well as for DTI and DKI metrics.

To compare the model fit of non-linear and linear models we used the Akaike information criterion (AIC)113 and Bayesian information criterion (BIC)114.

Brain age assessment

We estimated correlations across HBA and GBA to assess their similarities in addition to the model output provided from the prediction procedure. We also correlated age with the LI (see Eq. (1)) for the three modalities (dMRI, T1-weighted, multimodal MRI), and estimated the age sensitivity of the LI as described in (Eqs. (2)–(4)).

As preregistered (https://aspredicted.org/if5yr.pdf), to test the relationships between hemisphere (H), modality (M), and HBA while controlling for age, sex, and scanner site, we employed linear mixed effects regression (LMER) models of the following form:

$$\hat{HBA}=H+M+H\times M+Sex+Age+Sex\times Age+(1| Site)+(1| I),$$
(5)

whereIrefers to the random intercept at the level of the individual. Post-hoc group differences were observed for hemisphere, modality and their interaction.

Next, handedness (Ha) was added to the model to observe whether there are model differences between the resulting LMER:

$$\hat{HBA}=Ha+H\times Ha+H+M+H\times M+Sex+Age+Sex\times Age+(1| Site)+(1| I),$$
(6)

and the previous model. Models were statistically compared using LRTs112.

For sex-stratified analyses, we considered brain age estimates both from models using data from both sexes together, as well as models that were trained on females-only or males-only data. The modelling choice (MC) was included as a factor for the sex-stratified brain age analyses in the formula of Eq. (6).

Finally, the LIs (Eq. (1) of left and right brain age predictions for T1-weighted, diffusion and multimodal MRI (LIHBA, i.e. the asymmetry in brain age predictions) were associated with age, controlling for sex and scanner site as random effect:

$$\hat{Age}=L{I}_{HBA}+Sex+(1| Site).$$
(7)

The LIHBAs’ age-sensitivity was then assessed (as for brain features, see Eqs. (2)–(4)), using LRTs comparing the above model with a baseline model excluding LIHBA (Eq. (4)):

$$\hat{Age}=Sex+(1| Site).$$
(8)

This procedure was also done for each sex individually, also separating between brain age models predictions which were obtained from the data from both sexes compared to a single sex.

Phenotype associations of brain age

In an exploratory analysis step, we assessed association patterns between brain ages and health and lifestyle factors which have previously demonstrated an association with brain age20,26,38,39,40,41. This analysis step served to compare phenotype associations across estimated brain ages. The health and lifestyle factors included alcohol drinking (binary), height and weight supplementing body mass index (BMI), diabetes diagnosis (binary), diastolic blood pressure, systolic blood pressure, pulse pressure, hypertension (binary), cholesterol level (binary), and smoking (binary describing current smokers). For this last analysis step, LMERs were used with the following structure:

$$\hat{P}=BA+Sex+Age+Sex\times Age+(1| Site),$$
(9)

where BA refers brain age incorporating both GBA and HBA, P is the phenotype.

Furthermore, where applicable, we corrected p-values for multiple testing using Bonferroni correction and an α-level of p < 0.05. This involves multiplying the p-value by the number of tests used to test the same hypothesis. Adjusted p-values are marked as padj and unadjusted p-values as p. We used a high-precision approach to calculate exact p-values utilising the Multiple Precision Floating-Point Reliable R package115, and report standardised β-values. Sex and site were entered as independent factorial nominal variables in the applicable regression models, with sex being a binary (0 = female, 1 = male) and scanner site a multinominal (0 = Cheadle, 1 = Newcastle, 2 = Reading, 3 = Bristol). Finally, we repeated the presented statistical analyses stratifying for sex.

Effect size measures

For the estimation of the effect size of group differences we used Cohen’s d, estimated from the means of the two groups being compared (\({\bar{X}}_{1},{\bar{X}}_{2}\)) and the pooled standard deviation of the two groups (s):

$$d=\frac{{\bar{X}}_{1}-{\bar{X}}_{2}}{s}.$$

For the estimation of the effect size of associations, we used the Pearson correlation coefficient, which is estimated from two variables’ individual data points (xi, yi), and their averages (\(\bar{x},\bar{y}\)):

$$r=\frac{\sum ({x}_{i}-\bar{x})({y}_{i}-\bar{y})}{\sqrt{\sum {({x}_{i}-\bar{x})}^{2}\cdot \sum {({y}_{i}-\bar{y})}^{2}}}.$$

For multivariate associations, we used regression coefficients / β-weights from the respective regression equation (see Eq. (2)–(9)).

Reporting summary

Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.