Log in

Component awareness facilitates Chinese character recognition in non-native Chinese speakers: analysis of the multiple mediation effect

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigated the development of component awareness, semantic radical identification ability, semantic radical knowledge application, Chinese character recognition, and the relationship among these abilities in nonnative speakers. A total of 139 Vietnamese undergraduates majoring in Chinese language who were sorted according to Chinese learning time (second-year, third-year, and fourth-year students) took part in the present study and completed a delayed copy task, a semantic cue searching task, a sentence cloze task, and a Chinese character recognition test. The results showed that (1) semantic radical identification ability, semantic radical knowledge application, and Chinese character recognition improved with additional Chinese language study; (2) there were significant positive correlations among component awareness, semantic radical identification, semantic radical knowledge application, and Chinese character recognition; and (3) semantic radical identification ability and semantic radical knowledge application partially mediated the relationship between component awareness and Chinese character recognition after controlling for Chinese learning time and orthographic awareness. Component awareness impacts semantic radical knowledge application through semantic radical identification, which in turn influences Chinese character recognition. In other words, component awareness positively predicts Chinese character learning for nonnative Chinese learners.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. GB/T 12200.2: Chinese information processing—Vocabulary—Part 02: Chinese and Chinese character. (1994). State Language Affairs Commission, the Ministry of Education of the People's Republic of China. www.china-language.gov.cn

  2. GF 0014–2009: Specification of Common Modern Chinese Character Components and Component Names. (2009). State Language Affairs Commission, the Ministry of Education of the People's Republic of China. www.china-language.gov.cn

  3. Centre for Chinese Linguistics, Peking University, China: http://ccl.pku.edu.cn:8080/ccl_corpus/

References

  • Anderson, R. C., Ku, Y. M., Li, W., Chen, X., Wu, X., & Shu, H. (2013). Learning to see the patterns in Chinese characters. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(1), 41–56. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2012.689789

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H. C., Hsu, C. C., Chang, L. Y., Lin, Y. C., Chang, K. E., & Sung, Y. T. (2013). Using a radical-derived character e-learning platform to increase learner knowledge of Chinese characters. Language Learning and Technology, 17(1), 89–106. http://hdl.handle.net/10125/24511

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, L. B., & Siok, W. W. (1999). Semantic radicals contribute to the visual identification of Chinese characters. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(4), 559–576. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2629

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feng, L. P. (1998). An analysis report of pronunciation of 2905 Chinese characters used in teaching Chinese as a foreign language. Journal of Bei**g Normal University (china), 6, 94–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hao, M. L. (2007). The emergence and development of orthographic awareness among foreign learners of Chinese as a second language. Chinese Teaching in the World, 22(1), 35–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hao, M. L., & Fan, H. Q. (2008). The influence of properties of component and configuration of Chinese character on language learners’ character writing. Language Teaching and Linguistic Studies, 5, 24–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S. H., Ng, T. T., & Ng, W. K. (2003). A “radical” approach to reading development in Chinese: The role of semantic radicals and phonetic radicals. Journal of Literacy Research, 35(3), 849–878. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15548430jlr3503_3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hong, J. C., Wu, C. L., Chen, H. C., Chang, Y. L., & Chang, K. E. (2016). Effect of radical-position regularity for Chinese orthographic skills of Chinese-as-a-second-language learners. Computers in Human Behavior, 59, 402–410. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.02.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jiang, X., & Zhao, G. (2001). A survey on the strategies for learning Chinese characters among CSL beginners. Language Teaching and Linguistic Studies, 4, 10–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, F. (2005). A brief history of Chinese characters. China Friendship Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, H., Peng, H., & Shu, H. (2006). A study on the emergence and development of Chinese orthographic awareness in preschool and school Children. Psychological Development and Education, 22(1), 35–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, Y., Kang, J. S., Wei, L., & Zhang, S. Y. (1992). A study of semantic-phonetic compound characters in modern Chinese. Applied Linguistics, 1, 74–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, Y., Li, H., & Wang, M. (2020). The roles of phonological recoding, semantic radicals and writing practice in orthographic learning in Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 24(3), 252–263. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2019.1663199

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, N. N., Tong, W., & Yan, G. L. (2014). The development of Chinese orthographic awareness in pre-school children. Psychological Development and Education, 30(5), 457–465.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nguyen, T. P., Li, H., Wu, X. C., & Sun, P. (2016). The development of Chinese component awareness in Vietnamese students learning Chinese as a foreign language. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 14(1), 20–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nguyen, T. P., Zhang, J., Li, H., Wu, X., & Cheng, Y. (2017). Teaching semantic radicals facilitates inferring new character meaning in sentence reading for nonnative Chinese speakers. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1846. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01846

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pak, A. K., Cheng-Lai, A., Tso, I. F., Shu, H., Li, W., & Anderson, R. C. (2005). Visual chunking skills of Hong Kong children. Reading and Writing, 18(5), 437–454. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-6575-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274(5294), 1926–1928.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151–218. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shen, H. H., & Ke, C. (2007). Radical awareness and word acquisition among nonnative learners of Chinese. The Modern Language Journal, 91(1), 97–111. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2007.00511.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shi, B. J., Li, H., Zhang, Y. P., & Shu, H. (2011). The role of logographeme characteristics and orthographic awareness in low-grade children’s writing development. Psychological Development and Education, 27(3), 297–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shi, D. G., & Wan, Y. X. (1998). A survey on the teaching Chinese character as a second language. Language Teaching and Linguistic Studies, 1, 36–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H. (2003). Chinese writing system and learning to read. International Journal of Psychology, 38(5), 274–285. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590344000060

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., & Anderson, R. C. (1997). Role of radical awareness in the character and word acquisition of Chinese children. Reading Research Quarterly, 32(1), 78–89. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.32.1.5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Anderson, R. C., & Wu, N. (2000). Phonetic awareness: Knowledge of orthography-phonology relationships in the character acquisition of Chinese children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(1), 56–62. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-0663.92.1.56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., & Chung, K. (1999). Using radicals in teaching Chinese characters to second language learners. Psychologia, 42(4), 243–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., Zhu, X., & Peng, D. (1999). Positional specificity of radicals in Chinese character recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(4), 498–519. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2625

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • The Institute of Linguistics, The Chinese Academy of Social Sciences. (2005). Modern Chinese Dictionary (the 5th edition). Bei**g, China: The Commercial Press.

  • Tong, X., & McBride, C. (2014). Chinese children’s statistical learning of orthographic regularities: Positional constraints and character structure. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(4), 291–308. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2014.884098

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tong, X., & Yip, J. H. Y. (2015). Cracking the Chinese character: Radical sensitivity in learners of Chinese as a foreign language and its relationship to Chinese word reading. Reading and Writing, 28(2), 159–181. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9519-y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, M., Liu, Y., & Perfetti, C. A. (2004). The implicit and explicit learning of orthographic structure and function of a new writing system. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8(4), 357–379. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0804_3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, M., Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2003). Alphabetic readers quickly acquire orthographic structure in learning to read Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7(2), 183–208. https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0702_4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Y., Yin, L., & Mcbride, C. (2015). Unique predictors of early reading and writing: A one-year longitudinal study of Chinese kindergarteners. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 32, 51–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.02.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, C., & Bever, T. (2010). Chinese character decoding: A semantic bias? Reading and Writing, 23(5), 589–605. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9228-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yin, L., & McBride, C. (2015). Chinese kindergartners learn to read characters analytically. Psychological Science, 26(4), 424–432. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614567203

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, H., & Roberts, L. (2019). The role of phonological awareness, phonetic radical awareness in acquiring Chinese literacy skills in learners of Chinese as a second language. System, 81, 163–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2019.02.007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J., Li, H., Dong, Q., Xu, J., & Sholar, E. (2016). Implicit use of radicals in learning characters for nonnative learners of Chinese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(03), 507–527. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716415000090

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao, G., & Jiang, X. (2002). What is the most effective strategy for learning Chinese characters: A survey among CSL beginners. Applied Linguistics, 2, 79–85.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to **nchun Wu.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national res earch committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Nguyen, T.P., Li, H., Feng, J. et al. Component awareness facilitates Chinese character recognition in non-native Chinese speakers: analysis of the multiple mediation effect. Read Writ 36, 1685–1704 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-022-10353-8

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-022-10353-8

Keywords

Navigation