Log in

Eye movements cannot explain vibration-induced visual motion and motion aftereffect

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Eye movements are thought to account for a number of visual motion illusions involving stationary objects presented against a featureless background or apparent motion of the whole visual field. We tested two different versions of the eye movement account: (a) the retinal slip explanation and (b) the nystagmus-suppression explanation, in particular their ability to account for visual motion experienced during vibration of the neck muscles, and for the visual motion aftereffect following vibration. We vibrated the neck (ventral sternocleidomastoid muscles, bilaterally, or right dorsal muscles) and measured eye movements in conjunction with perceived illusory displacement of an LED presented in complete darkness (N=10). To test the retinal-slip explanation, we compared the direction of slow eye movements to the direction of illusory motion of the visual target. To test the suppression explanation, we estimated the direction of suppressed slow-phase eye movements and compared it to the direction of illusory motion. Two main findings show that neither actual nor suppressed eye movements cause the illusory motion and motion aftereffect. Firstly, eye movements do not reverse direction when the illusory motion reverses after vibration stops. Secondly, there are large individual differences with regards to the direction of eye movements in observers who all experience a similar visual illusion. We conclude that, rather than eye movements, a more global spatial constancy mechanism that takes into account head movement is responsible for the illusion. The results also argue against the notion of a single central signal that determines both perceptual experience and oculomotor behaviour.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price includes VAT (Germany)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Biguer B, Donaldson I, Hein A, Jeannerod M (1988) Neck muscle vibration modifies the representation of visual motion and direction in man. Brain 111:1405–1424

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beddell H, Patel S (2005) Attenuation of perceived motion smear during the vestibulo-ocular reflex. Vis Res 45:2191–2200

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chaudhuri A (1991) Eye movements and the motion aftereffect: alternatives to the induced motion hypothesis. Vis Res 31(9):1639–1645

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Clark B, Graybiel A (1949) The effect of angular acceleration on sound localization: the audiogyral illusion. J Physiol 28:235–44

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Evanoff J, Lackner J (1987) Influence of maintained ocular deviation on the spatial displacement component of the oculogyral illusion. Percept Psychophys 42:25–28

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Evanoff J, Lackner J (1988) Some proprioceptive influences on the spatial displacement component of the oculogyral illusion. Percept Psychophys 43:526–530

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman T, Sumnall J, Snowden R (2003) The extra-retinal motion aftereffect. J Vis 3:771–779

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin G, McCloskey D, Matthews P (1972) The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. Brain 95:705–748

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Graybiel A, Hupp D (1946) The oculo-gyral illusion. J Aviat Med 3:1–12

    Google Scholar 

  • Howard I (1982) Human visual orientation.Wiley, pp 273–294

  • Karnath H, Sievering D, Fetter M (1994) The interactive contribution of neck muscle proprioception and vestibular stimulation to subjective “straight ahead” orientation in man. Exp Brain Res 101:140–146

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lackner JR, DiZio P (2005) Vestibular, proprioceptive, and haptic contributions to spatial orientations. Annu Rev Psychol 56:115–147

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lackner J, Levine M (1979) Changes in apparent body orientation and sensory localization induced by vibration of postural muscles: vibratory myesthetic illusions. Aviat, Space Environ Med 50:346–354

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lennerstrand G, Han Y, Velay J-L (1996) Properties of eye movements induced by activation of neck muscle proprioceptors. Graefe’s Arch Clin Exp Opthalmol 234:703–709

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lewald J, Karnath H (2000) Vestibular influence on human auditory space perception. J Neurophysiol 84(2):1107–1111

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lewald J, Karnath H, Ehrenstein W (1999) Neck-proprioceptive influence on auditory lateralization. Exp Brain Res 125:389–396

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews P (1972) Where does Sherrington’s ‘muscular sense’ originate? Muscles, joints, corollary discharges? Annu Rev Neurosci 5:189–218

    Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey DI (1973) Differences between the senses of movement and position shown by the effects of loading and vibration of muscles in man. Brain Res Bull 63:119–131

    Google Scholar 

  • Mergner T, Rottler G, Kimming H, Becker W (1992) The role of vestibular and neck inputs for the perception of objects in space. Exp Brain Res 89:655–668

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moore ST, Curthoys IS, McCoy SG (1991) VTM-an image- processing system for measuring ocular torsion. Comput Methods Programs Biomed 35:219–230

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moore ST, Haslwanter T, Curthoys IS, Smith ST (1996) A geometric basis for measurement of three-dimensional eye position using image processing. Vis Res 36:445–459

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Peli E, Garcia-Perez M (2003) Motion perception during involuntary eye vibration. Exp Brain Res 149:431–438

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Popov K, Lekhel H, Faldon M, Bronstein A, Gresty M (1999) Visual and oculomotor responses induced by neck vibration in normal subjects and labyrinthine-defective patients. Exp Brain Res 128:343–352

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Raphan T, Matsuo V, Cohen B (1979) Velocity storage in the vestibulo-ocular reflex arc. Exp Brain Res 35:229–248

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Roll JP, Roll R, Velay J-L (1991) Proprioception as a link between body space and extra-personal space. Brain and space, Oxford University Press, pp 112–132

  • Taylor JL, McCloskey D (1991) Illusions of head and visual target displacement induced by vibration of neck muscles. Brain 114:755–759

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Velay J, Roll R, Lennerstrand G, Roll J (1994) Eye proprioception and visual localization in humans: influence of ocular dominance and visual context. Vis Res 34(16):2169–2176

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Whiteside T, Graybiel A, Niven J (1965) Visual illusions of movement. Brain 88:13–210

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young ED, Fernandez C, Goldberg JM (1977) Responses of squirrel monkey vestibular neurons to audio-frequency sound and head vibration. Acta Otolaryngol 84:352–360

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Nenad Petkovski for programming in LabView and Claire Benito for assistance with various aspects of research. This work was supported by University of Sydney SESQUI grant to TSC, and NHMRC grant to IC.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tatjana Seizova-Cajic.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Seizova-Cajic, T., Sachtler, W.L.B. & Curthoys, I.S. Eye movements cannot explain vibration-induced visual motion and motion aftereffect. Exp Brain Res 173, 141–152 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0373-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0373-2

Keywords

Navigation